[1]
J. Oostenbroek, ‘Comprehensive Longitudinal Study Challenges the Existence of Neonatal Imitation in Humans’, Current Biology, vol. 26, no. 10, pp. 1334–1338, 2016, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047.
[2]
U. Liszkowski, ‘Twelve-Month-Olds Point to Share Attention and Interest’, Developmental Science, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 297–307, 2004, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00349.x.
[3]
M. Tomasello and M. Carpenter, ‘Shared Intentionality’, Developmental Science, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 121–125, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x.
[4]
F. Warneken and M. Tomasello, ‘Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age’, Infancy, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 271–294, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x.
[5]
R. Hepach, ‘A New Look at Children’s Prosocial Motivation’, Infancy, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 67–90, 2013, doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00130.x.
[6]
M. Carpenter, ‘Twelve- and 18-Month-Olds Copy Actions in Terms of Goals’, Developmental Science, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. F13–F20, 2005, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00385.x.
[7]
H. Moll and M. Tomasello, ‘12- and 18-Month-Old Infants Follow Gaze to Spaces Behind Barriers’, Developmental Science, vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 1–9, 2004, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00315.x.
[8]
M. Tomasello, ‘A New Look at Infant Pointing’, Child Development, vol. 78, no. 3, pp. 705–722, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.x.
[9]
A. N. Meltzoff, ‘“Like Me”: A Foundation for Social Cognition’, Developmental Science, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 126–134, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.x.
[10]
P. Mitchell, ‘Acquiring a Theory of Mind’, in An Introduction to Developmental Psychology, 2nd Edition., Chichester: BPS Blackwell, 2011, pp. 357–384.
[11]
S. Baron-Cohen, ‘Does the Autistic Child Have a "Theory of Mind”?’, Cognition, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 37–46, 1985, doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.
[12]
H. M. Wellman, ‘Meta-Analysis of Theory-of-Mind Development: The Truth About False Belief’, Child Development, vol. 72, no. 3, pp. 655–684, 2001 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1132444
[13]
M. Yamaguchi, V. A. Kuhlmeier, K. Wynn, and K. vanMarle, ‘Continuity in Social Cognition From Infancy to Childhood’, Developmental Science, vol. 12, no. 5, pp. 746–752, 2009, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x.
[14]
A. A. Yazdi, T. P. German, M. A. Defeyter, and M. Siegal, ‘Competence and Performance in Belief-Desire Reasoning Across Two Cultures: The Truth, the Whole Truth and Nothing but the Truth About False Belief?’, Cognition, vol. 100, no. 2, pp. 343–368, 2006, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.004.
[15]
R. Banerjee, D. Watling, and M. Caputi, ‘Peer Relations and the Understanding of Faux Pas: Longitudinal Evidence for Bidirectional Associations’, Child Development, vol. 82, no. 6, pp. 1887–1905, 2011, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01669.x.
[16]
T. Callaghan, ‘Synchrony in the Onset of Mental-State Reasoning: Evidence From Five Cultures’, Psychological Science, vol. 16, no. 5, pp. 378–384, 2005, doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01544.x.
[17]
R. Ensor and C. Hughes, ‘Content or Connectedness? Mother–Child Talk and Early Social Understanding’, Child Development, vol. 79, no. 1, pp. 201–216, 2008, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01120.x.
[18]
C. C. Peterson, H. M. Wellman, and D. Liu, ‘Steps in Theory-of-Mind Development for Children With Deafness or Autism’, Child Development, vol. 76, no. 2, pp. 502–517, 2005, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00859.x.
[19]
T. Ruffman, ‘Older (But Not Younger) Siblings Facilitate False Belief Understanding.’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 161–174, 1998 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=1997-42746-013&site=ehost-live
[20]
C. Thoermer, B. Sodian, M. Vuori, H. Perst, and S. Kristen, ‘Continuity From an Implicit to an Explicit Understanding of False Belief From Infancy to Preschool Age’, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 172–187, 2012, doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x.
[21]
K. Durkin, ‘Social Cognition I: Understanding the Social World’, in Developmental Social Psychology: From Infancy to Old Age, Cambridge, Mass: Blackwell, 1995, pp. 287–301.
[22]
J. Kroger, M. Martinussen, and J. E. Marcia, ‘Identity Status Change During Adolescence and Young Adulthood: A Meta-Analysis’, Journal of Adolescence, vol. 33, no. 5, pp. 683–698, 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.11.002.
[23]
J. Ross, M. Yilmaz, R. Dale, R. Cassidy, I. Yildirim, and S. Zeedyk, ‘Cultural Differences in Self-Recognition: The Early Development of Autonomous and Related Selves?’, Developmental Science, vol. 20, no. 3, 2017, doi: 10.1111/desc.12387.
[24]
R. W. Robins and K. H. Trzesniewski, ‘Self-Esteem Development Across the Lifespan’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 158–162, 2005, doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00353.x.
[25]
C. Moore, ‘The Development of Body Self-Awareness’, Infancy, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 157–174, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00220.x.
[26]
M. Mann, ‘Self-Esteem in a Broad-Spectrum Approach for Mental Health Promotion’, Health Education Research, vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 357–372, 2004, doi: 10.1093/her/cyg041.
[27]
J. S. Phinney, ‘Ethnic Identity in Adolescents and Adults: Review of Research’, Psychological Bulletin, vol. 108, no. 3, pp. 499–514, 1990, doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.499. [Online]. Available: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=1991-07036-001&site=ehost-live
[28]
M. K. Rothbart, ‘Temperament and Personality: Origins and Outcomes’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 78, no. 1, pp. 122–135, 2000, doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. [Online]. Available: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=1999-15749-010&site=ehost-live
[29]
D. Clay, V. L. Vignoles, and H. Dittmar, ‘Body Image and Self-Esteem Among Adolescent Girls: Testing the Influence of Sociocultural Factors’, Journal of Research on Adolescence, vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 451–477, 2005, doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2005.00107.x.
[30]
H. Keller, ‘Parenting Styles and the Development of the Categorical Self: A Longitudinal Study on Mirror Self-Recognition in Cameroonian Nso and German Families’, International Journal of Behavioral Development, vol. 29, no. 6, pp. 496–504, 2005, doi: 10.1177/01650250500147485.
[31]
S. Denham, ‘Emotional and Social Development in Childhood’, in The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development, 2nd Edition., vol. Handbooks of Developmental Psychology, P. K. Smith and C. H. Hart, Eds. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, 2014, pp. 413–433.
[32]
P. Leman, ‘Emotional Development and Attachment’, in Developmental Psychology, Maidenhead: McGraw-Hill, 2012, pp. 157–195.
[33]
V. Bruce, R. N. Campbell, G. Doherty-Sneddon, S. Langton, S. McAuley, and R. Wright, ‘Testing Face Processing Skills in Children’, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 319–333, 2000, doi: 10.1348/026151000165715.
[34]
X. Gao and D. Maurer, ‘A Happy Story: Developmental Changes in Children’s Sensitivity to Facial Expressions of Varying Intensities’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 107, no. 2, pp. 67–86, 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.003.
[35]
C. M. Herba, ‘The Development of Emotion-Processing in Children: Effects of Age, Emotion, and Intensity’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol. 47, no. 11, pp. 1098–1106, 2006, doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01652.x.
[36]
C. Herba and M. Phillips, ‘Annotation: Development of Facial Expression Recognition From Childhood to Adolescence: Behavioural and Neurological Perspectives’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol. 45, no. 7, pp. 1185–1198, 2004, doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00316.x.
[37]
A. A. Marsh and N. Ambady, ‘The Influence of the Fear Facial Expression on Prosocial Responding’, Cognition & Emotion, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 225–247, 2007, doi: 10.1080/02699930600652234.
[38]
D. Watling, L. Workman, and V. J. Bourne, ‘Emotion Lateralisation: Developments Throughout the Lifespan’, Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 389–411, 2012 [Online]. Available: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/1357650X.2012.682160?src=recsys
[39]
L. Workman, L. Chilvers, H. Yeomans, and S. Taylor, ‘Development of Cerebral Lateralisation for Recognition of Emotions in Chimeric Faces in Children Aged 5 to 11’, Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, vol. 11, no. 6, pp. 493–507, 2006, doi: 10.1080/13576500600724963.
[40]
U. Goswami and P. Bryant, ‘Children’s Cognitive Development and Learning’. 2007 [Online]. Available: https://www.cne.psychol.cam.ac.uk/pdfs/publication-pdfs/Primary_Review_2-1a_report_CogDevLearn_Goswami-Bryant_2007.pdf
[41]
A. Liddard, A. M. Pinkham, and E. Smith, ‘Pretend Play and Cognitive Development’, in The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, 2nd ed., Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell, 2011.
[42]
A. Lillard, A. M. Pinkham, and E. Smith, ‘Pretend Play and Cognitive Development’, in The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, 2011 [Online]. Available: http://faculty.virginia.edu/ASLillard/PDFs/Lillard%20(2010).pdf
[43]
S. A. Gelman and M. Meyer, ‘Child Categorization’, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, vol. 2, no. 1, pp. 95–105, 2011, doi: 10.1002/wcs.96.
[44]
G. Westermann, D. Mareschal, M. H. Johnson, S. Sirois, M. W. Spratling, and M. S. C. Thomas, ‘Neuroconstructivism’, Developmental Science, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 75–83, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.x.
[45]
R. Mejía-Arauz, B. Rogoff, and R. Paradise, ‘Cultural Variation in Children’s Observation During a Demonstration’, International Journal of Behavioral Development, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 282–291, 2005, doi: 10.1177/01650250544000062.
[46]
R. Aslin and J. Fiser, ‘Methodological Challenges for Understanding Cognitive Development in Infants’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 92–98, 2005, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.003.
[47]
D. Bergen, ‘The Role of Pretend Play in Children’s Cognitive Development.’, Early Childhood Research & Practice, vol. 4, no. 1, 2002 [Online]. Available: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED464763
[48]
L. Ma and A. S. Lillard, ‘What Makes an Act a Pretense One? Young Children’s Pretend-Real Judgments and Explanations’, Child Development Research, vol. 2013, pp. 1–9, 2013, doi: 10.1155/2013/467872.
[49]
N. Althaus and G. Westermann, ‘Labels Constructively Shape Object Categories in 10-Month-Old Infants’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 151, pp. 5–17, 2016, doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.013.
[50]
G. Diesendruck and P. Bloom, ‘How Specific is the Shape Bias?’, Child Development, vol. 74, no. 1, pp. 168–178, 2003, doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00528.
[51]
E. A. Ware, ‘Individual and Developmental Differences in Preschoolers’ Categorization Biases and Vocabulary Across Tasks’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 153, pp. 35–56, 2017, doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.009.
[52]
S. R. Waxman and L. L. Namy, ‘Challenging the Notion of a Thematic Preference in Young Children.’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 33, no. 3, pp. 555–567, 1997, doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.3.555.
[53]
P. C. Quinn and P. D. Eimas, ‘Perceptual Cues That Permit Categorical Differentiation of Animal Species by Infants’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 63, no. 1, pp. 189–211, 1996, doi: 10.1006/jecp.1996.0047.
[54]
‘Spatial cognition’. .
[55]
M. Vasilyeva and S. F. Lourenco, ‘Development of Spatial Cognition’, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 349–362, 2012, doi: 10.1002/wcs.1171.
[56]
N. S. Newcombe, ‘The Nativist-Empiricist Controversy in the Context of Recent Research on Spatial and Quantitative Development’, Psychological Science, vol. 13, no. 5, pp. 395–401, 2002, doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00471.
[57]
‘Mathematical cognition’. .
[58]
I. Berteletti, D. Lucangeli, M. Piazza, S. Dehaene, and M. Zorzi, ‘Numerical Estimation in Preschoolers’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 46, no. 2, pp. 545–551, 2010, doi: 10.1037/a0017887.
[59]
L. Feigenson, S. Carey, and M. Hauser, ‘The Representations Underlying Infants’ Choice of More: Object Files Versus Analog Magnitudes’, Psychological Science, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 150–156, 2002, doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00427.
[60]
H. J. Broadbent, E. K. Farran, and A. Tolmie, ‘Egocentric and Allocentric Navigation Strategies in Williams Syndrome and Typical Development’, Developmental Science, vol. 17, no. 6, pp. 920–934, 2014, doi: 10.1111/desc.12176.
[61]
J. Bullens, K. Iglói, A. Berthoz, A. Postma, and L. Rondi-Reig, ‘Developmental Time Course of the Acquisition of Sequential Egocentric and Allocentric Navigation Strategies’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 107, no. 3, pp. 337–350, 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.010.
[62]
E. W. Bushnell, B. E. McKenzie, D. A. Lawrence, and S. Connell, ‘The Spatial Coding Strategies of One-Year-Old Infants in a Locomotor Search Task’, Child Development, vol. 66, no. 4, 1995, doi: 10.2307/1131790.
[63]
A. E. Learmonth, N. S. Newcombe, and J. Huttenlocher, ‘Toddlers’ Use of Metric Information and Landmarks to Reorient’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 80, no. 3, pp. 225–244, 2001, doi: 10.1006/jecp.2001.2635.
[64]
L. Hermer and E. S. Spelke, ‘A Geometric Process for Spatial Reorientation in Young Children’, Nature, vol. 370, no. 6484, pp. 57–59, 1994, doi: 10.1038/370057a0.
[65]
A. E. Learmonth, L. Nadel, and N. S. Newcombe, ‘Children’s Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory’, Psychological Science, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 337–341, 2002, doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00461.x.
[66]
M. Nardini, N. Burgess, K. Breckenridge, and J. Atkinson, ‘Differential Developmental Trajectories for Egocentric, Environmental and Intrinsic Frames of Reference in Spatial Memory’, Cognition, vol. 101, no. 1, pp. 153–172, 2006, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.09.005.
[67]
N. S. Newcombe, S. C. Levine, and K. S. Mix, ‘Thinking About Quantity: The Intertwined Development of Spatial and Numerical Cognition’, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, vol. 6, no. 6, pp. 491–505, 2015, doi: 10.1002/wcs.1369.
[68]
S. M. Pruden, S. C. Levine, and J. Huttenlocher, ‘Children’s Spatial Thinking: Does Talk About the Spatial World Matter?’, Developmental Science, vol. 14, no. 6, pp. 1417–1430, 2011, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01088.x.
[69]
J. Sluzenski, N. S. Newcombe, and E. Satlow, ‘Knowing Where Things Are in the Second Year of Life: Implications for Hippocampal Development’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 16, no. 8, pp. 1443–1451, 2004, doi: 10.1162/0898929042304804.
[70]
S. E. Antell and D. P. Keating, ‘Perception of Numerical Invariance in Neonates’, Child Development, vol. 54, no. 3, 1983, doi: 10.2307/1130057.
[71]
H. Barth, K. La Mont, J. Lipton, and E. S. Spelke, ‘Abstract Number and Arithmetic in Preschool Children’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 102, no. 39, pp. 14116–14121, 2005, doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505512102.
[72]
V. Bermejo, S. Morales, and J. G. deOsuna, ‘Supporting Children’s Development of Cardinality Understanding’, Learning and Instruction, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 381–398, 2004, doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.010.
[73]
J. F. Cantlon, E. M. Brannon, E. J. Carter, and K. A. Pelphrey, ‘Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children’, PLoS Biology, vol. 4, no. 5, 2006, doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040125.
[74]
C. Chen and H. W. Stevenson, ‘Motivation and Mathematics Achievement: A Comparative Study of Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East Asian High School Students’, Child Development, vol. 66, no. 4, 1995, doi: 10.2307/1131808.
[75]
L. B. Cohen and K. S. Marks, ‘How Infants Process Addition and Subtraction Events’, Developmental Science, vol. 5, no. 2, pp. 186–201, 2002, doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00220.
[76]
D. C. Hyde, D. A. Boas, C. Blair, and S. Carey, ‘Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Shows Right Parietal Specialization for Number in Pre-Verbal Infants’, NeuroImage, vol. 53, no. 2, pp. 647–652, 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.030.
[77]
K. F. Miller and J. W. Stigler, ‘Counting in Chinese: Cultural Variation in a Basic Cognitive Skill’, Cognitive Development, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 279–305, 1987, doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(87)90091-8.
[78]
K. Wynn, ‘Addition and Subtraction by Human Infants’, Nature, vol. 358, no. 6389, pp. 749–750, 1992, doi: 10.1038/358749a0.
[79]
A. Castles, K. Rastle, and K. Nation, ‘Ending the Reading Wars: Reading Acquisition From Novice to Expert’, Psychological Science in the Public Interest, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 5–51, 2018, doi: 10.1177/1529100618772271.
[80]
K. Cain, Reading Development and Difficulties, vol. BPS Textbooks in Psychology. Chichester, West Sussex, U.K.: BPS Blackwell/John Wiley, 2010.
[81]
C. Hulme and M. J. Snowling, ‘Reading Disorders I: Developmental Dyslexia’, in Developmental Disorders of Language Learning and Cognition, Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 2009, pp. 37–89.
[82]
C. Hulme and M. J. Snowling, ‘Reading Disorders II: Reading Comprehension Impairment’, in Developmental Disorders of Language Learning and Cognition, Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 2009.
[83]
P. J. Clarke, ‘Ameliorating Children’s Reading-Comprehension Difficulties: A Randomized Controlled Trial’, Psychological Science, vol. 21, no. 8, pp. 1106–1116, 2010, doi: 10.1177/0956797610375449.
[84]
F. J. Duff and P. J. Clarke, ‘Practitioner Review: Reading Disorders: What Are the Effective Interventions and How Should They Be Implemented and Evaluated?’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol. 52, no. 1, pp. 3–12, 2011, doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02310.x.
[85]
V. Muter, C. Hulme, M. J. Snowling, and J. Stevenson, ‘Phonemes, Rimes, Vocabulary, and Grammatical Skills as Foundations of Early Reading Development: Evidence From a Longitudinal Study.’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 40, no. 5, pp. 665–681, 2004 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2004-17950-002&site=ehost-live
[86]
J. R. Kirby and R. S. Savage, ‘Can the Simple View Deal With the Complexities of Reading?’, Literacy, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 75–82, 2008, doi: 10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00487.x.
[87]
K. Nation, ‘Working Memory Deficits in Poor Comprehenders Reflect Underlying Language Impairments’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 73, no. 2, pp. 139–158, 1999, doi: 10.1006/jecp.1999.2498.
[88]
K. Nation and C. Hulme, ‘Learning to Read Changes Children’s Phonological Skills: Evidence From a Latent Variable Longitudinal Study of Reading and Nonword Repetition’, Developmental Science, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 649–659, 2011, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01008.x.
[89]
K. Nation, ‘A Longitudinal Investigation of Early Reading and Language Skills in Children With Poor Reading Comprehension’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol. 51, no. 9, pp. 1031–1039, 2010, doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02254.x.
[90]
D. Powell, ‘Does the PMSP Connectionist Model of Single Word Reading Learn to Read in the Same Way as a Child?’, Journal of Research in Reading, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 229–250, 2006, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00300.x.
[91]
J. Ricketts, ‘Research Review: Reading Comprehension in Developmental Disorders of Language and Communication’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol. 52, no. 11, pp. 1111–1123, 2011, doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02438.x.
[92]
M. Stuart, ‘Literacy as a Complex Activity: Deconstructing the Simple View of Reading’, Literacy, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 59–66, 2008, doi: 10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00490.x.
[93]
W. E. Tunmer and J. W. Chapman, ‘The Simple View of Reading Redux: Vocabulary Knowledge and the Independent Components Hypothesis’, Journal of Learning Disabilities, vol. 45, no. 5, pp. 453–466, 2012, doi: 10.1177/0022219411432685.
[94]
M. J. Adams, Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning About Print. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 1990.
[95]
K. Cain, ‘Investigating the Causes of Reading Comprehension Failure: The Comprehension-Age Match Designl’, Reading and Writing, vol. 12, no. 1/2, pp. 31–40, 2000, doi: 10.1023/A:1008058319399.
[96]
C. Marshall, ‘Rapid Auditory Processing and Phonological Ability in Normal Readers and Readers With Dyslexia.’, Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, vol. 44, no. 4, pp. 925–940, 2001 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=cms&AN=5813581&site=ehost-live
[97]
S. M. McHale, ‘The Family Contexts of Gender Development in Childhood and Adolescence’, Social Development, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 125–148, 2003, doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00225.
[98]
D. Joel, ‘Sex Beyond the Genitalia: The Human Brain Mosaic’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 112, no. 50, pp. 15468–15473, 2015, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509654112.
[99]
D. N. Ruble, ‘Gender Constancy and the Effects of Sex-Typed Televised Toy Commercials’, Child Development, vol. 52, no. 2, pp. 667–673, 1981, doi: 10.2307/1129188.
[100]
C. L. Martin and D. Ruble, ‘Children’s Search for Gender Cues’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 67–70, 2004, doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x.
[101]
K. Shutts, ‘Social Categories Guide Young Children’s Preferences for Novel Objects’, Developmental Science, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 599–610, 2009, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00913.x.
[102]
J. S. Hyde, ‘Gender Similarities and Differences’, Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 373–398, 2014, doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057.
[103]
M. Ingalhalikar, ‘Sex Differences in the Structural Connectome of the Human Brain’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 111, no. 2, pp. 823–828, 2014, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1316909110.
[104]
D. Joel and R. Tarrasch, ‘On the Mis-Presentation and Misinterpretation of Gender-Related Data: The Case of Ingalhalikar’s Human Connectome Study’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 111, no. 6, pp. E637–E637, 2014, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1323319111.
[105]
E. E. Maccoby, ‘Perspectives on Gender Development’, International Journal of Behavioral Development, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 398–406, 2000, doi: 10.1080/016502500750037946.
[106]
B. Muzzatti, ‘Gender and Mathematics: Attitudes and Stereotype Threat Susceptibility in Italian Children.’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 43, no. 3, pp. 747–759, 2007 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2007-06280-017&site=ehost-live
[107]
S.-J. Blakemore and K. L. Mills, ‘Is Adolescence a Sensitive Period for Sociocultural Processing?’, Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 187–207, 2014, doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202.
[108]
L. Steinberg, ‘Cognitive and Affective Development in Adolescence’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 9, no. 2, pp. 69–74, 2005, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005.
[109]
L. Steinberg, ‘A Social Neuroscience Perspective on Adolescent Risk-Taking’, Developmental Review, vol. 28, no. 1, pp. 78–106, 2008, doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002.
[110]
M. Gardner, ‘Peer Influence on Risk Taking, Risk Preference, and Risky Decision Making in Adolescence and Adulthood: An Experimental Study.’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 41, no. 4, pp. 625–635, 2005 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2005-08221-004&site=ehost-live
[111]
S. Choudhury, ‘Social Cognitive Development During Adolescence’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, vol. 1, no. 3, pp. 165–174, 2006, doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl024.
[112]
B. Luna, ‘Maturation of Cognitive Processes From Late Childhood to Adulthood’, Child Development, vol. 75, no. 5, pp. 1357–1372, 2004, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00745.x.
[113]
J. Mendle, ‘Detrimental Psychological Outcomes Associated With Early Pubertal Timing in Adolescent Girls’, Developmental Review, vol. 27, no. 2, pp. 151–171, 2007, doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2006.11.001.
[114]
L. H. Somerville, ‘The Teenage Brain: Sensitivity to Social Evaluation’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 121–127, 2013, doi: 10.1177/0963721413476512.
[115]
C. J. Hooper, ‘Adolescents’ Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: Implications for the Development of Decision Making and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex.’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 40, no. 6, pp. 1148–1158, 2004 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2004-20098-018&site=ehost-live
[116]
B. Hare and M. Tomasello, ‘Chimpanzees Are More Skilful in Competitive Than in Cooperative Cognitive Tasks’, Animal Behaviour, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 571–581, 2004, doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.011.
[117]
L. M. Hopper, S. P. Lambeth, S. J. Schapiro, and A. Whiten, ‘Observational Learning in Chimpanzees and Children Studied Through “Ghost” Conditions’, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 275, no. 1636, pp. 835–840, 2008, doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1542.
[118]
V. Kersken, J.-C. Gómez, U. Liszkowski, A. Soldati, and C. Hobaiter, ‘A Gestural Repertoire of 1- to 2-Year-Old Human Children: In Search of the Ape Gestures’, Animal Cognition, vol. 22, pp. 577–595, 2018, doi: 10.1007/s10071-018-1213-z.
[119]
C. Krupenye, F. Kano, S. Hirata, J. Call, and M. Tomasello, ‘Great Apes Anticipate That Other Individuals Will Act According to False Beliefs’, Science, vol. 354, no. 6308, pp. 110–114, 2016, doi: 10.1126/science.aaf8110.
[120]
J. M. Plotnik, F. B. M. de Waal, and D. Reiss, ‘Self-Recognition in an Asian Elephant’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 103, no. 45, pp. 17053–17057, 2006, doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608062103.
[121]
M. Tomasello and M. Carpenter, ‘Shared Intentionality’, Developmental Science, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 121–125, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x.
[122]
J. Call, B. Agnetta, and M. Tomasello, ‘Cues That Chimpanzees Do and Do Not Use to Find Hidden Objects’, Animal Cognition, vol. 3, no. 1, pp. 23–34, 2000, doi: 10.1007/s100710050047.
[123]
J. Call and M. Tomasello, ‘A Nonverbal False Belief Task: The Performance of Children and Great Apes’, Child Development, vol. 70, no. 2, pp. 381–395, 1999, doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00028.
[124]
J. Call and M. Tomasello, ‘Does the Chimpanzee Have a Theory of Mind? 30 Years Later’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 12, no. 5, pp. 187–192, 2008, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010.
[125]
H. Prior, A. Schwarz, and O. Güntürkün, ‘Mirror-Induced Behavior in the Magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of Self-Recognition’, PLoS Biology, vol. 6, no. 8, 2008, doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060202.
[126]
F. Range, Z. Viranyi, and L. Huber, ‘Selective Imitation in Domestic Dogs’, Current Biology, vol. 17, no. 10, pp. 868–872, 2007, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.04.026.