[1]
Adams, M.J. 1990. Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning About Print. MIT Press.
[2]
Althaus, N. and Westermann, G. 2016. Labels Constructively Shape Object Categories in 10-Month-Old Infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 151, (2016), 5–17. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.013.
[3]
Antell, S.E. and Keating, D.P. 1983. Perception of Numerical Invariance in Neonates. Child Development. 54, 3 (1983). DOI:https://doi.org/10.2307/1130057.
[4]
Aslin, R. and Fiser, J. 2005. Methodological Challenges for Understanding Cognitive Development in Infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9, 3 (2005), 92–98. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.003.
[5]
Banerjee, R. et al. 2011. Peer Relations and the Understanding of Faux Pas: Longitudinal Evidence for Bidirectional Associations. Child Development. 82, 6 (2011), 1887–1905. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01669.x.
[6]
Baron-Cohen, S. 1985. Does the Autistic Child Have a "Theory of Mind”? Cognition. 21, 1 (1985), 37–46. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.
[7]
Barth, H. et al. 2005. Abstract Number and Arithmetic in Preschool Children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102, 39 (2005), 14116–14121. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0505512102.
[8]
Bergen, D. 2002. The Role of Pretend Play in Children’s Cognitive Development. Early Childhood Research & Practice. 4, 1 (2002).
[9]
Bermejo, V. et al. 2004. Supporting Children’s Development of Cardinality Understanding. Learning and Instruction. 14, 4 (2004), 381–398. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.010.
[10]
Berteletti, I. et al. 2010. Numerical Estimation in Preschoolers. Developmental Psychology. 46, 2 (2010), 545–551. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017887.
[11]
Blakemore, S.-J. and Mills, K.L. 2014. Is Adolescence a Sensitive Period for Sociocultural Processing? Annual Review of Psychology. 65, 1 (2014), 187–207. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202.
[12]
Broadbent, H.J. et al. 2014. Egocentric and Allocentric Navigation Strategies in Williams Syndrome and Typical Development. Developmental Science. 17, 6 (2014), 920–934. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12176.
[13]
Bruce, V. et al. 2000. Testing Face Processing Skills in Children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 18, 3 (2000), 319–333. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1348/026151000165715.
[14]
Bullens, J. et al. 2010. Developmental Time Course of the Acquisition of Sequential Egocentric and Allocentric Navigation Strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 107, 3 (2010), 337–350. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.010.
[15]
Bushnell, E.W. et al. 1995. The Spatial Coding Strategies of One-Year-Old Infants in a Locomotor Search Task. Child Development. 66, 4 (1995). DOI:https://doi.org/10.2307/1131790.
[16]
Cain, K. 2000. Investigating the Causes of Reading Comprehension Failure: The Comprehension-Age Match Designl. Reading and Writing. 12, 1/2 (2000), 31–40. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008058319399.
[17]
Cain, K. 2010. Reading Development and Difficulties. BPS Blackwell/John Wiley.
[18]
Call, J. et al. 2000. Cues That Chimpanzees Do and Do Not Use to Find Hidden Objects. Animal Cognition. 3, 1 (2000), 23–34. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1007/s100710050047.
[19]
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. 1999. A Nonverbal False Belief Task: The Performance of Children and Great Apes. Child Development. 70, 2 (1999), 381–395. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00028.
[20]
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. 2008. Does the Chimpanzee Have a Theory of Mind? 30 Years Later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 12, 5 (2008), 187–192. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010.
[21]
Callaghan, T. 2005. Synchrony in the Onset of Mental-State Reasoning: Evidence From Five Cultures. Psychological Science. 16, 5 (2005), 378–384. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01544.x.
[22]
Cantlon, J.F. et al. 2006. Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children. PLoS Biology. 4, 5 (2006). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040125.
[23]
Carpenter, M. 2005. Twelve- and 18-Month-Olds Copy Actions in Terms of Goals. Developmental Science. 8, 1 (2005), F13–F20. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00385.x.
[24]
Castles, A. et al. 2018. Ending the Reading Wars: Reading Acquisition From Novice to Expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 19, 1 (2018), 5–51. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100618772271.
[25]
Chen, C. and Stevenson, H.W. 1995. Motivation and Mathematics Achievement: A Comparative Study of Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East Asian High School Students. Child Development. 66, 4 (1995). DOI:https://doi.org/10.2307/1131808.
[26]
Choudhury, S. 2006. Social Cognitive Development During Adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 1, 3 (2006), 165–174. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl024.
[27]
Clarke, P.J. 2010. Ameliorating Children’s Reading-Comprehension Difficulties: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Psychological Science. 21, 8 (2010), 1106–1116. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610375449.
[28]
Clay, D. et al. 2005. Body Image and Self-Esteem Among Adolescent Girls: Testing the Influence of Sociocultural Factors. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 15, 4 (2005), 451–477. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2005.00107.x.
[29]
Cohen, L.B. and Marks, K.S. 2002. How Infants Process Addition and Subtraction Events. Developmental Science. 5, 2 (2002), 186–201. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00220.
[30]
Denham, S. 2014. Emotional and Social Development in Childhood. The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. P.K. Smith and C.H. Hart, eds. Wiley-Blackwell. 413–433.
[31]
Diesendruck, G. and Bloom, P. 2003. How Specific is the Shape Bias? Child Development. 74, 1 (2003), 168–178. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00528.
[32]
Duff, F.J. and Clarke, P.J. 2011. Practitioner Review: Reading Disorders: What Are the Effective Interventions and How Should They Be Implemented and Evaluated? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 52, 1 (2011), 3–12. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02310.x.
[33]
Durkin, K. 1995. Social Cognition I: Understanding the Social World. Developmental Social Psychology: From Infancy to Old Age. Blackwell. 287–301.
[34]
Ensor, R. and Hughes, C. 2008. Content or Connectedness? Mother–Child Talk and Early Social Understanding. Child Development. 79, 1 (2008), 201–216. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01120.x.
[35]
Feigenson, L. et al. 2002. The Representations Underlying Infants’ Choice of More: Object Files Versus Analog Magnitudes. Psychological Science. 13, 2 (2002), 150–156. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00427.
[36]
Gao, X. and Maurer, D. 2010. A Happy Story: Developmental Changes in Children’s Sensitivity to Facial Expressions of Varying Intensities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 107, 2 (2010), 67–86. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.003.
[37]
Gardner, M. 2005. Peer Influence on Risk Taking, Risk Preference, and Risky Decision Making in Adolescence and Adulthood: An Experimental Study. Developmental Psychology. 41, 4 (2005), 625–635.
[38]
Gelman, S.A. and Meyer, M. 2011. Child Categorization. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2, 1 (2011), 95–105. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.96.
[39]
Goswami, U. and Bryant, P. 2007. Children’s Cognitive Development and Learning.
[40]
Hare, B. and Tomasello, M. 2004. Chimpanzees Are More Skilful in Competitive Than in Cooperative Cognitive Tasks. Animal Behaviour. 68, 3 (2004), 571–581. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.011.
[41]
Hepach, R. 2013. A New Look at Children’s Prosocial Motivation. Infancy. 18, 1 (2013), 67–90. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00130.x.
[42]
Herba, C. and Phillips, M. 2004. Annotation: Development of Facial Expression Recognition From Childhood to Adolescence: Behavioural and Neurological Perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 45, 7 (2004), 1185–1198. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00316.x.
[43]
Herba, C.M. 2006. The Development of Emotion-Processing in Children: Effects of Age, Emotion, and Intensity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 47, 11 (2006), 1098–1106. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01652.x.
[44]
Hermer, L. and Spelke, E.S. 1994. A Geometric Process for Spatial Reorientation in Young Children. Nature. 370, 6484 (1994), 57–59. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/370057a0.
[45]
Hooper, C.J. 2004. Adolescents’ Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: Implications for the Development of Decision Making and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. Developmental Psychology. 40, 6 (2004), 1148–1158.
[46]
Hopper, L.M. et al. 2008. Observational Learning in Chimpanzees and Children Studied Through ‘Ghost’ Conditions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 275, 1636 (2008), 835–840. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1542.
[47]
Hulme, C. and Snowling, M.J. 2009. Reading Disorders I: Developmental Dyslexia. Developmental Disorders of Language Learning and Cognition. Wiley-Blackwell. 37–89.
[48]
Hulme, C. and Snowling, M.J. 2009. Reading Disorders II: Reading Comprehension Impairment. Developmental Disorders of Language Learning and Cognition. Wiley-Blackwell.
[49]
Hyde, D.C. et al. 2010. Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Shows Right Parietal Specialization for Number in Pre-Verbal Infants. NeuroImage. 53, 2 (2010), 647–652. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.030.
[50]
Hyde, J.S. 2014. Gender Similarities and Differences. Annual Review of Psychology. 65, 1 (2014), 373–398. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057.
[51]
Ingalhalikar, M. 2014. Sex Differences in the Structural Connectome of the Human Brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111, 2 (2014), 823–828. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1316909110.
[52]
Joel, D. 2015. Sex Beyond the Genitalia: The Human Brain Mosaic. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 112, 50 (2015), 15468–15473. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1509654112.
[53]
Joel, D. and Tarrasch, R. 2014. On the Mis-Presentation and Misinterpretation of Gender-Related Data: The Case of Ingalhalikar’s Human Connectome Study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111, 6 (2014), E637–E637. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1323319111.
[54]
Keller, H. 2005. Parenting Styles and the Development of the Categorical Self: A Longitudinal Study on Mirror Self-Recognition in Cameroonian Nso and German Families. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 29, 6 (2005), 496–504. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/01650250500147485.
[55]
Kersken, V. et al. 2018. A Gestural Repertoire of 1- to 2-Year-Old Human Children: In Search of the Ape Gestures. Animal Cognition. 22, (2018), 577–595. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1213-z.
[56]
Kirby, J.R. and Savage, R.S. 2008. Can the Simple View Deal With the Complexities of Reading? Literacy. 42, 2 (2008), 75–82. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00487.x.
[57]
Kroger, J. et al. 2010. Identity Status Change During Adolescence and Young Adulthood: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Adolescence. 33, 5 (2010), 683–698. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.11.002.
[58]
Krupenye, C. et al. 2016. Great Apes Anticipate That Other Individuals Will Act According to False Beliefs. Science. 354, 6308 (2016), 110–114. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf8110.
[59]
Learmonth, A.E. et al. 2002. Children’s Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory. Psychological Science. 13, 4 (2002), 337–341. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00461.x.
[60]
Learmonth, A.E. et al. 2001. Toddlers’ Use of Metric Information and Landmarks to Reorient. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 80, 3 (2001), 225–244. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2001.2635.
[61]
Leman, P. 2012. Emotional Development and Attachment. Developmental Psychology. McGraw-Hill. 157–195.
[62]
Liddard, A. et al. 2011. Pretend Play and Cognitive Development. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Wiley-Blackwell.
[63]
Lillard, A. et al. 2011. Pretend Play and Cognitive Development. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development.
[64]
Liszkowski, U. 2004. Twelve-Month-Olds Point to Share Attention and Interest. Developmental Science. 7, 3 (2004), 297–307. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00349.x.
[65]
Luna, B. 2004. Maturation of Cognitive Processes From Late Childhood to Adulthood. Child Development. 75, 5 (2004), 1357–1372. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00745.x.
[66]
Ma, L. and Lillard, A.S. 2013. What Makes an Act a Pretense One? Young Children’s Pretend-Real Judgments and Explanations. Child Development Research. 2013, (2013), 1–9. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/467872.
[67]
Maccoby, E.E. 2000. Perspectives on Gender Development. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 24, 4 (2000), 398–406. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/016502500750037946.
[68]
Mann, M. 2004. Self-Esteem in a Broad-Spectrum Approach for Mental Health Promotion. Health Education Research. 19, 4 (2004), 357–372. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/her/cyg041.
[69]
Marsh, A.A. and Ambady, N. 2007. The Influence of the Fear Facial Expression on Prosocial Responding. Cognition & Emotion. 21, 2 (2007), 225–247. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600652234.
[70]
Marshall, C. 2001. Rapid Auditory Processing and Phonological Ability in Normal Readers and Readers With Dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 44, 4 (2001), 925–940.
[71]
Martin, C.L. and Ruble, D. 2004. Children’s Search for Gender Cues. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 13, 2 (2004), 67–70. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x.
[72]
McHale, S.M. 2003. The Family Contexts of Gender Development in Childhood and Adolescence. Social Development. 12, 1 (2003), 125–148. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00225.
[73]
Mejía-Arauz, R. et al. 2005. Cultural Variation in Children’s Observation During a Demonstration. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 29, 4 (2005), 282–291. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/01650250544000062.
[74]
Meltzoff, A.N. 2007. ‘Like Me’: A Foundation for Social Cognition. Developmental Science. 10, 1 (2007), 126–134. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.x.
[75]
Mendle, J. 2007. Detrimental Psychological Outcomes Associated With Early Pubertal Timing in Adolescent Girls. Developmental Review. 27, 2 (2007), 151–171. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2006.11.001.
[76]
Miller, K.F. and Stigler, J.W. 1987. Counting in Chinese: Cultural Variation in a Basic Cognitive Skill. Cognitive Development. 2, 3 (1987), 279–305. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(87)90091-8.
[77]
Mitchell, P. 2011. Acquiring a Theory of Mind. An Introduction to Developmental Psychology. BPS Blackwell. 357–384.
[78]
Moll, H. and Tomasello, M. 2004. 12- and 18-Month-Old Infants Follow Gaze to Spaces Behind Barriers. Developmental Science. 7, 1 (2004), 1–9. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00315.x.
[79]
Moore, C. 2007. The Development of Body Self-Awareness. Infancy. 11, 2 (2007), 157–174. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00220.x.
[80]
Muter, V. et al. 2004. Phonemes, Rimes, Vocabulary, and Grammatical Skills as Foundations of Early Reading Development: Evidence From a Longitudinal Study. Developmental Psychology. 40, 5 (2004), 665–681.
[81]
Muzzatti, B. 2007. Gender and Mathematics: Attitudes and Stereotype Threat Susceptibility in Italian Children. Developmental Psychology. 43, 3 (2007), 747–759.
[82]
Nardini, M. et al. 2006. Differential Developmental Trajectories for Egocentric, Environmental and Intrinsic Frames of Reference in Spatial Memory. Cognition. 101, 1 (2006), 153–172. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.09.005.
[83]
Nation, K. 2010. A Longitudinal Investigation of Early Reading and Language Skills in Children With Poor Reading Comprehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 51, 9 (2010), 1031–1039. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02254.x.
[84]
Nation, K. 1999. Working Memory Deficits in Poor Comprehenders Reflect Underlying Language Impairments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 73, 2 (1999), 139–158. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1999.2498.
[85]
Nation, K. and Hulme, C. 2011. Learning to Read Changes Children’s Phonological Skills: Evidence From a Latent Variable Longitudinal Study of Reading and Nonword Repetition. Developmental Science. 14, 4 (2011), 649–659. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01008.x.
[86]
Newcombe, N.S. 2002. The Nativist-Empiricist Controversy in the Context of Recent Research on Spatial and Quantitative Development. Psychological Science. 13, 5 (2002), 395–401. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00471.
[87]
Newcombe, N.S. et al. 2015. Thinking About Quantity: The Intertwined Development of Spatial and Numerical Cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 6, 6 (2015), 491–505. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1369.
[88]
Oostenbroek, J. 2016. Comprehensive Longitudinal Study Challenges the Existence of Neonatal Imitation in Humans. Current Biology. 26, 10 (2016), 1334–1338. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047.
[89]
Peterson, C.C. et al. 2005. Steps in Theory-of-Mind Development for Children With Deafness or Autism. Child Development. 76, 2 (2005), 502–517. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00859.x.
[90]
Phinney, J.S. 1990. Ethnic Identity in Adolescents and Adults: Review of Research. Psychological Bulletin. 108, 3 (1990), 499–514. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.499.
[91]
Plotnik, J.M. et al. 2006. Self-Recognition in an Asian Elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 103, 45 (2006), 17053–17057. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0608062103.
[92]
Powell, D. 2006. Does the PMSP Connectionist Model of Single Word Reading Learn to Read in the Same Way as a Child? Journal of Research in Reading. 29, 2 (2006), 229–250. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00300.x.
[93]
Prior, H. et al. 2008. Mirror-Induced Behavior in the Magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of Self-Recognition. PLoS Biology. 6, 8 (2008). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0060202.
[94]
Pruden, S.M. et al. 2011. Children’s Spatial Thinking: Does Talk About the Spatial World Matter? Developmental Science. 14, 6 (2011), 1417–1430. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01088.x.
[95]
Quinn, P.C. and Eimas, P.D. 1996. Perceptual Cues That Permit Categorical Differentiation of Animal Species by Infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 63, 1 (1996), 189–211. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0047.
[96]
Range, F. et al. 2007. Selective Imitation in Domestic Dogs. Current Biology. 17, 10 (2007), 868–872. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.04.026.
[97]
Ricketts, J. 2011. Research Review: Reading Comprehension in Developmental Disorders of Language and Communication. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 52, 11 (2011), 1111–1123. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02438.x.
[98]
Robins, R.W. and Trzesniewski, K.H. 2005. Self-Esteem Development Across the Lifespan. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 14, 3 (2005), 158–162. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00353.x.
[99]
Ross, J. et al. 2017. Cultural Differences in Self-Recognition: The Early Development of Autonomous and Related Selves? Developmental Science. 20, 3 (2017). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12387.
[100]
Rothbart, M.K. 2000. Temperament and Personality: Origins and Outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78, 1 (2000), 122–135. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122.
[101]
Ruble, D.N. 1981. Gender Constancy and the Effects of Sex-Typed Televised Toy Commercials. Child Development. 52, 2 (1981), 667–673. DOI:https://doi.org/10.2307/1129188.
[102]
Ruffman, T. 1998. Older (But Not Younger) Siblings Facilitate False Belief Understanding. Developmental Psychology. 34, 1 (1998), 161–174.
[103]
Shutts, K. 2009. Social Categories Guide Young Children’s Preferences for Novel Objects. Developmental Science. 13, 4 (2009), 599–610. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00913.x.
[104]
Sluzenski, J. et al. 2004. Knowing Where Things Are in the Second Year of Life: Implications for Hippocampal Development. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 16, 8 (2004), 1443–1451. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042304804.
[105]
Somerville, L.H. 2013. The Teenage Brain: Sensitivity to Social Evaluation. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 22, 2 (2013), 121–127. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413476512.
[106]
Steinberg, L. 2008. A Social Neuroscience Perspective on Adolescent Risk-Taking. Developmental Review. 28, 1 (2008), 78–106. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002.
[107]
Steinberg, L. 2005. Cognitive and Affective Development in Adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9, 2 (2005), 69–74. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005.
[108]
Stuart, M. 2008. Literacy as a Complex Activity: Deconstructing the Simple View of Reading. Literacy. 42, 2 (2008), 59–66. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00490.x.
[109]
Thoermer, C. et al. 2012. Continuity From an Implicit to an Explicit Understanding of False Belief From Infancy to Preschool Age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 30, 1 (2012), 172–187. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x.
[110]
Tomasello, M. 2007. A New Look at Infant Pointing. Child Development. 78, 3 (2007), 705–722. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.x.
[111]
Tomasello, M. and Carpenter, M. 2007. Shared Intentionality. Developmental Science. 10, 1 (2007), 121–125. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x.
[112]
Tomasello, M. and Carpenter, M. 2007. Shared Intentionality. Developmental Science. 10, 1 (2007), 121–125. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x.
[113]
Tunmer, W.E. and Chapman, J.W. 2012. The Simple View of Reading Redux: Vocabulary Knowledge and the Independent Components Hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 45, 5 (2012), 453–466. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219411432685.
[114]
Vasilyeva, M. and Lourenco, S.F. 2012. Development of Spatial Cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 3, 3 (2012), 349–362. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1171.
[115]
Ware, E.A. 2017. Individual and Developmental Differences in Preschoolers’ Categorization Biases and Vocabulary Across Tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 153, (2017), 35–56. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.009.
[116]
Warneken, F. and Tomasello, M. 2007. Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age. Infancy. 11, 3 (2007), 271–294. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x.
[117]
Watling, D. et al. 2012. Emotion Lateralisation: Developments Throughout the Lifespan. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 17, 4 (2012), 389–411.
[118]
Waxman, S.R. and Namy, L.L. 1997. Challenging the Notion of a Thematic Preference in Young Children. Developmental Psychology. 33, 3 (1997), 555–567. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.3.555.
[119]
Wellman, H.M. 2001. Meta-Analysis of Theory-of-Mind Development: The Truth About False Belief. Child Development. 72, 3 (2001), 655–684.
[120]
Westermann, G. et al. 2007. Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science. 10, 1 (2007), 75–83. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.x.
[121]
Workman, L. et al. 2006. Development of Cerebral Lateralisation for Recognition of Emotions in Chimeric Faces in Children Aged 5 to 11. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 11, 6 (2006), 493–507. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500600724963.
[122]
Wynn, K. 1992. Addition and Subtraction by Human Infants. Nature. 358, 6389 (1992), 749–750. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/358749a0.
[123]
Yamaguchi, M. et al. 2009. Continuity in Social Cognition From Infancy to Childhood. Developmental Science. 12, 5 (2009), 746–752. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x.
[124]
Yazdi, A.A. et al. 2006. Competence and Performance in Belief-Desire Reasoning Across Two Cultures: The Truth, the Whole Truth and Nothing but the Truth About False Belief? Cognition. 100, 2 (2006), 343–368. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.004.
[125]
Mathematical cognition.
[126]
Spatial cognition.