[1]
S. P. Ahmed, A. Bittencourt-Hewitt, and C. L. Sebastian, ‘Neurocognitive Bases of Emotion Regulation Development in Adolescence’, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 15, pp. 11–25, 2015, doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.006.
[2]
S.-J. Blakemore and T. W. Robbins, ‘Decision-Making in the Adolescent Brain’, Nature Neuroscience, vol. 15, no. 9, pp. 1184–1191, 2012, doi: 10.1038/nn.3177.
[3]
S.-J. Blakemore and K. L. Mills, ‘Is Adolescence a Sensitive Period for Sociocultural Processing?’, Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 187–207, 2014, doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202.
[4]
M. H. Johnson and R. Griffin, ‘The Emergence of the Social Brain Network: Evidence From Typical and Atypical Development’, Development and Psychopathology, vol. 17, no. 03, 2005, doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050297.
[5]
T. Paus, M. Keshavan, and J. N. Giedd, ‘Why Do Many Psychiatric Disorders Emerge During Adolescence?’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2010, doi: 10.1038/nrn2513.
[6]
J. F. Cantlon, P. Pinel, S. Dehaene, and K. A. Pelphrey, ‘Cortical Representations of Symbols, Objects, and Faces Are Pruned Back during Early Childhood’, Cerebral Cortex, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 191–199, 2011, doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq078.
[7]
J. Chein and D. Albert, ‘Peers Increase Adolescent Risk Taking by Enhancing Activity in the Brain’s Reward Circuitry’, Developmental Science, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. F1–F10, 2011, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x.
[8]
T. A. Hare and N. Tottenham, ‘Biological Substrates of Emotional Reactivity and Regulation in Adolescence During an Emotional Go-Nogo Task’, Biological Psychiatry, vol. 63, no. 10, pp. 927–934, 2008, doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.015.
[9]
C. Sebastian, E. Viding, K. D. Williams, and S.-J. Blakemore, ‘Social Brain Development and the Affective Consequences of Ostracism in Adolescence’, Brain and Cognition, vol. 72, no. 1, pp. 134–145, 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.008.
[10]
P. Shaw and N. J. Kabani, ‘Neurodevelopmental Trajectories of the Human Cerebral Cortex’, Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 28, no. 14, pp. 3586–3594, 2008, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008.
[11]
W. Mischel et al., ‘“Willpower” Over the Life Span: Decomposing Self-Regulation’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 252–256, 2011, doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq081.
[12]
P. Anderson, ‘Assessment and Development of Executive Function (EF) During Childhood’, Child Neuropsychology, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 71–82, 2002, doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724.
[13]
P. J. Anderson and N. Reidy, ‘Assessing Executive Function in Preschoolers’, Neuropsychology Review, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 345–360, 2012, doi: 10.1007/s11065-012-9220-3.
[14]
S. A. Bunge and S. B. Wright, ‘Neurodevelopmental Changes in Working Memory and Cognitive Control’, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 243–250, 2007, doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.005.
[15]
A. Diamond and K. Lee, ‘Interventions Shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4 to 12 Years Old’, Science, vol. 333, no. 6045, pp. 959–964, 2011, doi: 10.1126/science.1204529.
[16]
M. Melby-Lervåg and C. Hulme, ‘Is Working Memory Training Effective? a Meta-Analytic Review’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 49, no. 2, pp. 270–291, 2013, doi: 10.1037/a0028228. [Online]. Available: http://web.a.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail/detail?vid=1&sid=c6b2f891-e842-42ab-9f25-4e895af7f4ac%40sessionmgr4006&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#AN=2012-12954-001&db=pdh
[17]
TW. . Watts, G. J. Duncan, and H. Quan, ‘Revisiting the Marshmallow Test: A Conceptual Replication Investigating Links Between Early Delay of Gratification and Later Outcomes.’, 2018 [Online]. Available: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0956797618761661
[18]
C. Kidd, H. Palmeri, and R. N. Aslin, ‘Rational Snacking: Young Children’s Decision-Making on the Marshmallow Task Is Moderated by Beliefs About Environmental Reliability’, Cognition, vol. 126, no. 1, pp. 109–114, 2013, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.004.
[19]
B. Lamm et al., ‘Waiting for the Second Treat: Developing Culture-Specific Modes of Self-Regulation’, Child Development, vol. 89, no. 3, pp. e261–e277, 2018, doi: 10.1111/cdev.12847.
[20]
S. M. Carlson et al., ‘Cohort Effects in Children’s Delay of Gratification’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 54, no. 8, pp. 1395–1407, 2018, doi: 10.1037/dev0000533. [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2018-29923-001&site=ehost-live
[21]
B. J. Casey and L. H. Somerville, ‘Behavioral and Neural Correlates of Delay of Gratification 40 Years Later’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, vol. 108, no. 36, 2011 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/27979415?seq=1#page_scan_tab_contents
[22]
V. A. Anderson and P. Anderson, ‘Development of Executive Functions Through Late Childhood and Adolescence in an Australian Sample’, Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 385–406, 2001, doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_5.
[23]
H. Kwon, A. L. Reiss, and V. Menon, ‘Neural Basis of Protracted Developmental Changes in Visuo-Spatial Working Memory’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, vol. 99, no. 20, 2002 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3073397?seq=1#page_scan_tab_contents
[24]
M. Huizinga, C. V. Dolan, and M. W. van der Molen, ‘Age-Related Change in Executive Function: Developmental Trends and a Latent Variable Analysis’, Neuropsychologia, vol. 44, no. 11, pp. 2017–2036, 2006, doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010.
[25]
T. E. Moffitt and L. Arseneault, ‘A Gradient of Childhood Self-Control Predicts Health, Wealth, and Public Safety’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 108, no. 7, pp. 2693–2698, 2011, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108.
[26]
M. M. Haith, ‘Who Put the Cog in Infant Cognition? Is Rich Interpretation Too Costly?’, Infant Behavior and Development, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 167–179, 1998, doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90001-7.
[27]
J. L. Shinskey and Y. Munakata, ‘Something Old, Something New: A Developmental Transition From Familiarity to Novelty Preferences With Hidden Objects’, Developmental Science, vol. 13, no. 2, 2010, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00899.x.
[28]
J. L. Shinskey, ‘The Sound of Darkness: Why Do Auditory Cues Aid Infants’ Search for Objects Hidden by Darkness but Not by Visible Occluders?’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 44, no. 6, pp. 1715–1725, 2008 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2008-16008-015&site=ehost-live
[29]
J. Shinskey and Y. Munakata, ‘Are Infants in the Dark About Hidden Objects?’, Developmental Science, vol. 6, no. 3, pp. 273–282, 2003 [Online]. Available: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1467-7687.00283/abstract
[30]
J. L. Shinskey and Y. Munakata, ‘Detecting Transparent Barriers: Clear Evidence Against the Means-End Deficit Account of Search Failures’, Infancy, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 395–404, 2001, doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0203_7.
[31]
E. S. Spelke, ‘Nativism, Empiricism, and the Origins of Knowledge’, Infant Behavior and Development, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 181–200, 1998, doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90002-9.
[32]
R. Banerjee and N. Yuill, ‘Children’s Understanding of Self-Presentational Display Rules: Associations With Mental-State Understanding’, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 111–124, 1999, doi: 10.1348/026151099165186.
[33]
C. Kelsey, T. Grossmann, and A. Vaish, ‘Early Reputation Management: Three-Year-Old Children Are More Generous Following Exposure to Eyes’, Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 9, 2018, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00698.
[34]
J. M. Engelmann and D. J. Rapp, ‘The Influence of Reputational Concerns on Children’s Prosociality’, Current Opinion in Psychology, vol. 20, pp. 92–95, 2018, doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.024.
[35]
J. M. Engelmann, E. Herrmann, and M. Tomasello, ‘Five-Year Olds, but Not Chimpanzees, Attempt to Manage Their Reputations’, PLoS ONE, vol. 7, no. 10, 2012, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048433.
[36]
G. Fu and K. Lee, ‘Social Grooming in the Kindergarten: The Emergence of Flattery Behavior’, Developmental Science, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 255–265, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00583.x.
[37]
K. L. Leimgruber, A. Shaw, L. R. Santos, and K. R. Olson, ‘Young Children Are More Generous when Others Are Aware of Their Actions’, PLoS ONE, vol. 7, no. 10, 2012, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048292.
[38]
D. Albert, J. Chein, and L. Steinberg, ‘The Teenage Brain’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 114–120, 2013, doi: 10.1177/0963721412471347.
[39]
C. Tennie, U. Frith, and C. D. Frith, ‘Reputation Management in the Age of the World-Wide Web’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 14, no. 11, pp. 482–488, 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.003.
[40]
G. Fu, G. D. Heyman, M. Qian, T. Guo, and K. Lee, ‘Young Children With a Positive Reputation to Maintain Are Less Likely to Cheat’, Developmental Science, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 275–283, 2016, doi: 10.1111/desc.12304.
[41]
E. Cage, G. Bird, and E. Pellicano, ‘Reputation Management in Children on the Autism Spectrum’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 46, no. 12, pp. 3798–3811, 2016, doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2923-1.
[42]
P. Rochat, T. Broesch, and K. Jayne, ‘Social Awareness and Early Self-Recognition’, Consciousness and Cognition, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 1491–1497, 2012, doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.007.
[43]
J. S. DeLoache, ‘Becoming Symbol-Minded’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 66–70, 2004, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.004.
[44]
J. L. Shinskey and L. J. Jachens, ‘Picturing Objects in Infancy’, Child Development, pp. 1813–1820, 2014, doi: 10.1111/cdev.12243.
[45]
G. A. Strouse, A. Nyhout, and P. A. Ganea, ‘The Role of Book Features in Young Children’s Transfer of Information from Picture Books to Real-World Contexts’, Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 9, 2018, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00050.
[46]
E. Adi-Japha, I. Levin, and S. Solomon, ‘Emergence of Representation in Drawing: The Relation Between Kinematic and Referential Aspects’, Cognitive Development, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 25–51, 1998, doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90019-3.
[47]
A. E. Berti and N. H. Freeman, ‘Representational Change in Resources for Pictorial Innovation: A Three-Component Analysis’, Cognitive Development, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. 501–522, 1997, doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90020-4.
[48]
A. Karmiloff-Smith, ‘Constraints on Representational Change: Evidence From Children’s Drawing’, Cognition, vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 57–83, 1990, doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90031-E.
[49]
S. Morra, ‘Cognitive Aspects of Change in Drawings: A Neo-Piagetian Theoretical Account’, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 317–341, 2005, doi: 10.1348/026151005X27182.
[50]
A. M. J. Silk and G. V. Thomas, ‘Development and Differentiation in Children’s Figure Drawings’, British Journal of Psychology, vol. 77, no. 3, pp. 399–410, 1986, doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02206.x.
[51]
F. Spensley and J. Taylor, ‘The Development of Cognitive Flexibility: Evidence From Children’s Drawings’, Human Development, vol. 42, no. 6, pp. 300–324, 1999, doi: 10.1159/000022639. [Online]. Available: https://search-proquest-com.ezproxy01.rhul.ac.uk/docview/224018166?OpenUrlRefId=info:xri/sid:primo&accountid=11455
[52]
K. A. Dunfield, ‘A Construct Divided: Prosocial Behavior as Helping, Sharing, and Comforting Subtypes’, Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 5, 2014, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00958.
[53]
J. K. Hamlin, K. Wynn, and P. Bloom, ‘Social Evaluation by Preverbal Infants’, Nature, vol. 450, no. 7169, pp. 557–559, 2007, doi: 10.1038/nature06288.
[54]
A. Martin and K. R. Olson, ‘Beyond Good and Evil: What Motivations Underlie Children’s Prosocial Behavior?’, Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 159–175, 2015, doi: 10.1177/1745691615568998.
[55]
A. Vaish, M. Carpenter, and M. Tomasello, ‘Sympathy Through Affective Perspective Taking and Its Relation to Prosocial Behavior in Toddlers’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 534–543, 2009, doi: 10.1037/a0014322. [Online]. Available: http://web.a.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail/detail?vid=1&sid=f0edf3c4-8ba9-4c07-8dbb-282a048d99cd%40sessionmgr4009&hid=4114&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#AN=2009-02701-019&db=pdh
[56]
F. Warneken and M. Tomasello, ‘Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age’, Infancy, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 271–294, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x.
[57]
W. van den Bos, M. Westenberg, E. van Dijk, and E. A. Crone, ‘Development of Trust and Reciprocity in Adolescence’, Cognitive Development, vol. 25, no. 1, pp. 90–102, 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.07.004.
[58]
P. R. Blake and K. McAuliffe, ‘"I Had So Much It Didn’t Seem Fair”: Eight-Year-Olds Reject Two Forms of Inequity’, Cognition, vol. 120, no. 2, pp. 215–224, 2011, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.006.
[59]
F. Margoni, ‘Infants’ Evaluation of Prosocial and Antisocial Agents: A Meta-Analysis’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 54, no. 8, pp. 1445–1455, 2018 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2018-33347-001&site=ehost-live
[60]
M. F. H. Schmidt and J. A. Sommerville, ‘Fairness Expectations and Altruistic Sharing in 15-Month-Old Human Infants’, PLoS ONE, vol. 6, no. 10, 2011, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023223.
[61]
A. Vaish, M. Carpenter, and M. Tomasello, ‘Young Children Selectively Avoid Helping People With Harmful Intentions’, Child Development, vol. 81, no. 6, pp. 1661–1669, 2010, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01500.x.
[62]
P. J. Bauer, ‘A Complementary Processes Account of the Development of Childhood Amnesia and a Personal Past’, Psychological Review, vol. 122, no. 2, pp. 204–231, 2015, doi: 10.1037/a0038939. [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2015-14440-002&site=ehost-live
[63]
H. Hayne, ‘Infant Memory Development: Implications for Childhood Amnesia’, Developmental Review, vol. 24, no. 1, pp. 33–73, 2004, doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2003.09.007.
[64]
F. Jack, ‘Maternal Reminiscing Style During Early Childhood Predicts the Age of Adolescents’ Earliest Memories.(Report)’, Child Development, vol. 80, no. 2, pp. 496–505, 2009 [Online]. Available: https://www.jstor.org/stable/29738629
[65]
K. Nelson and R. Fivush, ‘The Emergence of Autobiographical Memory: A Social Cultural Developmental Theory’, Psychological Review, vol. 111, no. 2, pp. 486–511, 2004, doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.486. [Online]. Available: http://web.b.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail/detail?vid=0&sid=bb7e5ab7-bbc4-45c7-8381-48973cc230f4%40sessionmgr1&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#AN=2004-12248-009&db=pdh
[66]
K. Tustin and H. Hayne, ‘Defining the Boundary: Age-Related Changes in Childhood Amnesia.’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 46, no. 5, pp. 1049–1061, Sep. 2010, doi: 10.1037/a0020105.
[67]
Q. Wang, ‘Infantile Amnesia Reconsidered: A Cross-Cultural Analysis’, Memory, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 65–80, 2003, doi: 10.1080/741938173.
[68]
P. Smith, ‘Play and Peer Relations’, in An Introduction to Developmental Psychology, 2nd ed., vol. BPS Textbooks in Psychology, Chichester: BPS Blackwell, 2011, pp. 453–485.
[69]
A. Bourchier and A. Davis, ‘Children’s Understanding of the Pretence-Reality Distinction: A Review of Current Theory and Evidence’, Developmental Science, vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 397–413, 2002, doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00236_1.
[70]
R. A. Dore, E. D. Smith, and A. S. Lillard, ‘How Is Theory of Mind Useful? Perhaps to Enable Social Pretend Play’, Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 6, 2015, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01559.
[71]
W. L. Haight and X.-L. Wang, ‘Universal, Developmental, and Variable Aspects of Young Children’s Play: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Pretending at Home’, Child Development, vol. 70, no. 6, pp. 1477–88, 1999 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1132319
[72]
A. Lillard, ‘Pretend Play as Twin Earth: A Social-Cognitive Analysis’, Developmental Review, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 495–531, 2001, doi: 10.1006/drev.2001.0532.
[73]
K. H. Rubin, K. S. Watson, and T. W. Jambor, ‘Free-Play Behaviors in Preschool and Kindergarten Children’, Child Development, vol. 49, no. 2, 1978, doi: 10.2307/1128725.