[1]
Adi-Japha, E. et al. 1998. Emergence of Representation in Drawing: The Relation Between Kinematic and Referential Aspects. Cognitive Development. 13, 1 (1998), 25–51. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90019-3.
[2]
Ahmed, S.P. et al. 2015. Neurocognitive Bases of Emotion Regulation Development in Adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 15, (2015), 11–25. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.006.
[3]
Anderson, P. 2002. Assessment and Development of Executive Function (EF) During Childhood. Child Neuropsychology. 8, 2 (2002), 71–82. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724.
[4]
Anderson, P.J. and Reidy, N. 2012. Assessing Executive Function in Preschoolers. Neuropsychology Review. 22, 4 (2012), 345–360. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-012-9220-3.
[5]
Anderson, V.A. and Anderson, P. 2001. Development of Executive Functions Through Late Childhood and Adolescence in an Australian Sample. Developmental Neuropsychology. 20, 1 (2001), 385–406. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN2001_5.
[6]
Bauer, P.J. 2015. A Complementary Processes Account of the Development of Childhood Amnesia and a Personal Past. Psychological Review. 122, 2 (2015), 204–231. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038939.
[7]
Berti, A.E. and Freeman, N.H. 1997. Representational Change in Resources for Pictorial Innovation: A Three-Component Analysis. Cognitive Development. 12, 4 (1997), 501–522. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90020-4.
[8]
Blakemore, S.-J. and Mills, K.L. 2014. Is Adolescence a Sensitive Period for Sociocultural Processing? Annual Review of Psychology. 65, 1 (2014), 187–207. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202.
[9]
Blakemore, S.-J. and Robbins, T.W. 2012. Decision-Making in the Adolescent Brain. Nature Neuroscience. 15, 9 (2012), 1184–1191. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3177.
[10]
Bourchier, A. and Davis, A. 2002. Children’s Understanding of the Pretence-Reality Distinction: A Review of Current Theory and Evidence. Developmental Science. 5, 4 (2002), 397–413. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00236_1.
[11]
de Boysson-Bardies, B. and Vihman, M.M. 1991. Adaptation to Language: Evidence from Babbling and First Words in Four Languages. Language. 67, 2 (1991). DOI:https://doi.org/10.2307/415108.
[12]
Bunge, S.A. and Wright, S.B. 2007. Neurodevelopmental Changes in Working Memory and Cognitive Control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 17, 2 (2007), 243–250. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.005.
[13]
Cantlon, J.F. et al. 2011. Cortical Representations of Symbols, Objects, and Faces Are Pruned Back during Early Childhood. Cerebral Cortex. 21, 1 (2011), 191–199. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq078.
[14]
Carlson, S.M. et al. 2018. Cohort Effects in Children’s Delay of Gratification. Developmental Psychology. 54, 8 (2018), 1395–1407. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000533.
[15]
Casey, B.J. and Somerville, L.H. 2011. Behavioral and Neural Correlates of Delay of Gratification 40 Years Later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States. 108, 36 (2011).
[16]
Chein, J. and Albert, D. 2011. Peers Increase Adolescent Risk Taking by Enhancing Activity in the Brain’s Reward Circuitry. Developmental Science. 14, 2 (2011), F1–F10. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x.
[17]
Cristia, A. et al. 2016. Test-Retest Reliability in Infant Speech Perception Tasks. Infancy. 21, 5 (2016), 648–667. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12127.
[18]
Danielson, D.K. et al. 2017. The Organization and Reorganization of Audiovisual Speech Perception in the First Year of Life. Cognitive Development. 42, (2017), 37–48. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.004.
[19]
Danielson, D.K. et al. 2017. The Organization and Reorganization of Audiovisual Speech Perception in the First Year of Life. Cognitive Development. 42, (2017), 37–48. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.004.
[20]
DeLoache, J.S. 2004. Becoming Symbol-Minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 8, 2 (2004), 66–70. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.004.
[21]
Diamond, A. and Lee, K. 2011. Interventions Shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4 to 12 Years Old. Science. 333, 6045 (2011), 959–964. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204529.
[22]
Dore, R.A. et al. 2015. How Is Theory of Mind Useful? Perhaps to Enable Social Pretend Play. Frontiers in Psychology. 6, (2015). DOI:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01559.
[23]
Goffin, C. and Ansari, D. 2019. How Are Symbols and Nonsymbolic Numerical Magnitudes Related? Exploring Bidirectional Relationships in Early Numeracy. Mind, Brain, and Education. 13, 3 (2019), 143–156. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12206.
[24]
Haight, W.L. and Wang, X.-L. 1999. Universal, Developmental, and Variable Aspects of Young Children’s Play: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Pretending at Home. Child Development. 70, 6 (1999), 1477–88.
[25]
Haith, M.M. 1998. Who Put the Cog in Infant Cognition? Is Rich Interpretation Too Costly? Infant Behavior and Development. 21, 2 (1998), 167–179. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90001-7.
[26]
Hare, T.A. and Tottenham, N. 2008. Biological Substrates of Emotional Reactivity and Regulation in Adolescence During an Emotional Go-Nogo Task. Biological Psychiatry. 63, 10 (2008), 927–934. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.015.
[27]
Hayne, H. 2004. Infant Memory Development: Implications for Childhood Amnesia. Developmental Review. 24, 1 (2004), 33–73. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2003.09.007.
[28]
Huizinga, M. et al. 2006. Age-Related Change in Executive Function: Developmental Trends and a Latent Variable Analysis. Neuropsychologia. 44, 11 (2006), 2017–2036. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010.
[29]
Izard, V. et al. 2009. Newborn Infants Perceive Abstract Numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106, 25 (2009), 10382–10385. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0812142106.
[30]
Jack, F. 2009. Maternal Reminiscing Style During Early Childhood Predicts the Age of Adolescents’ Earliest Memories.(Report). Child Development. 80, 2 (2009), 496–505.
[31]
Johnson, M.H. and Griffin, R. 2005. The Emergence of the Social Brain Network: Evidence From Typical and Atypical Development. Development and Psychopathology. 17, 03 (2005). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050297.
[32]
Jusczyk, P.W. et al. 1993. Infants′ Sensitivity to the Sound Patterns of Native Language Words. Journal of Memory and Language. 32, 3 (1993), 402–420. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1022.
[33]
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1990. Constraints on Representational Change: Evidence From Children’s Drawing. Cognition. 34, 1 (1990), 57–83. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90031-E.
[34]
Kidd, C. et al. 2013. Rational Snacking: Young Children’s Decision-Making on the Marshmallow Task Is Moderated by Beliefs About Environmental Reliability. Cognition. 126, 1 (2013), 109–114. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.004.
[35]
Kwon, H. et al. 2002. Neural Basis of Protracted Developmental Changes in Visuo-Spatial Working Memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States. 99, 20 (2002).
[36]
Lamm, B. et al. 2018. Waiting for the Second Treat: Developing Culture-Specific Modes of Self-Regulation. Child Development. 89, 3 (2018), e261–e277. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12847.
[37]
Libertus, M.E. et al. 2011. Preschool Acuity of the Approximate Number System Correlates With School Math Ability. Developmental Science. 14, 6 (2011), 1292–1300. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x.
[38]
Lillard, A. 2001. Pretend Play as Twin Earth: A Social-Cognitive Analysis. Developmental Review. 21, 4 (2001), 495–531. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.2001.0532.
[39]
Lyons, I.M. et al. 2018. Symbolic Number Skills Predict Growth in Nonsymbolic Number Skills in Kindergarteners. Developmental Psychology. 54, 3 (2018), 440–457. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000445.
[40]
May, L. et al. 2011. Language and the Newborn Brain: Does Prenatal Language Experience Shape the Neonate Neural Response to Speech? Frontiers in Psychology. 2, (2011). DOI:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00222.
[41]
Melby-Lervåg, M. and Hulme, C. 2013. Is Working Memory Training Effective? a Meta-Analytic Review. Developmental Psychology. 49, 2 (2013), 270–291. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028228.
[42]
Mischel, W. et al. 2011. ‘Willpower’ Over the Life Span: Decomposing Self-Regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 6, 2 (2011), 252–256. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq081.
[43]
Moffitt, T.E. and Arseneault, L. 2011. A Gradient of Childhood Self-Control Predicts Health, Wealth, and Public Safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108, 7 (2011), 2693–2698. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108.
[44]
Morra, S. 2005. Cognitive Aspects of Change in Drawings: A Neo-Piagetian Theoretical Account. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 23, 3 (2005), 317–341. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X27182.
[45]
Paus, T. et al. 2010. Why Do Many Psychiatric Disorders Emerge During Adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. (2010). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2513.
[46]
Petitto, L. and Marentette, P. 1991. Babbling in the Manual Mode: Evidence for the Ontogeny of Language. Science. 251, 5000 (1991), 1493–1496. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2006424.
[47]
Rubin, K.H. et al. 1978. Free-Play Behaviors in Preschool and Kindergarten Children. Child Development. 49, 2 (1978). DOI:https://doi.org/10.2307/1128725.
[48]
Saffran, J.R. et al. 1996. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants. Science. 274, 5294 (1996), 1926–1928. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.274.5294.1926.
[49]
Saffran, J.R. and Kirkham, N.Z. 2018. Infant Statistical Learning. Annual Review of Psychology. 69, 1 (2018), 181–203. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011805.
[50]
Sebastian, C. et al. 2010. Social Brain Development and the Affective Consequences of Ostracism in Adolescence. Brain and Cognition. 72, 1 (2010), 134–145. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.008.
[51]
Shaw, P. and Kabani, N.J. 2008. Neurodevelopmental Trajectories of the Human Cerebral Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 28, 14 (2008), 3586–3594. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008.
[52]
Shinskey, J. and Munakata, Y. 2003. Are Infants in the Dark About Hidden Objects? Developmental Science. 6, 3 (2003), 273–282.
[53]
Shinskey, J.L. 2008. The Sound of Darkness: Why Do Auditory Cues Aid Infants’ Search for Objects Hidden by Darkness but Not by Visible Occluders? Developmental Psychology. 44, 6 (2008), 1715–1725.
[54]
Shinskey, J.L. and Jachens, L.J. 2014. Picturing Objects in Infancy. Child Development. (2014), 1813–1820. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12243.
[55]
Shinskey, J.L. and Munakata, Y. 2001. Detecting Transparent Barriers: Clear Evidence Against the Means-End Deficit Account of Search Failures. Infancy. 2, 3 (2001), 395–404. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0203_7.
[56]
Shinskey, J.L. and Munakata, Y. 2010. Something Old, Something New: A Developmental Transition From Familiarity to Novelty Preferences With Hidden Objects. Developmental Science. 13, 2 (2010). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00899.x.
[57]
Siegler, R.S. 2016. Magnitude Knowledge: The Common Core of Numerical Development. Developmental Science. 19, 3 (2016), 341–361. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12395.
[58]
Silk, A.M.J. and Thomas, G.V. 1986. Development and Differentiation in Children’s Figure Drawings. British Journal of Psychology. 77, 3 (1986), 399–410. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02206.x.
[59]
Skeide, M.A. and Friederici, A.D. 2016. The Ontogeny of the Cortical Language Network. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 17, 5 (2016), 323–332. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.23.
[60]
Smith, P. 2017. Play and the Beginning of Peer Relations. An Introduction to Developmental Psychology. A. Slater and G. Bremner, eds. John Wiley & Sons. 477–506.
[61]
Spelke, E.S. 1998. Nativism, Empiricism, and the Origins of Knowledge. Infant Behavior and Development. 21, 2 (1998), 181–200. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90002-9.
[62]
Spensley, F. and Taylor, J. 1999. The Development of Cognitive Flexibility: Evidence From Children’s Drawings. Human Development. 42, 6 (1999), 300–324. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1159/000022639.
[63]
Spensley, F. and Taylor, J. The Development of Cognitive Flexibility: Evidence From Children’s Drawings. Human Development. 42, 6, 300–324.
[64]
Strouse, G.A. et al. 2018. The Role of Book Features in Young Children’s Transfer of Information from Picture Books to Real-World Contexts. Frontiers in Psychology. 9, (2018). DOI:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00050.
[65]
Thompson, B.N. and Goldstein, T.R. 2019. Disentangling Pretend Play Measurement: Defining the Essential Elements and Developmental Progression of Pretense. Developmental Review. 52, (2019), 24–41. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2019.100867.
[66]
Tustin, K. and Hayne, H. 2010. Defining the Boundary: Age-Related Changes in Childhood Amnesia. Developmental Psychology. 46, 5 (Sep. 2010), 1049–1061. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020105.
[67]
Vouloumanos, A. and Werker, J.F. 2007. Listening to Language at Birth: Evidence for a Bias for Speech in Neonates. Developmental Science. 10, 2 (2007), 159–164. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.x.
[68]
Wang, Q. 2003. Infantile Amnesia Reconsidered: A Cross-Cultural Analysis. Memory. 11, 1 (2003), 65–80. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/741938173.
[69]
Watts, TW.. et al. 2018. Revisiting the Marshmallow Test: A Conceptual Replication Investigating Links Between Early Delay of Gratification and Later Outcomes. (2018).
[70]
Werker, J.F. and Hensch, T.K. 2015. Critical Periods in Speech Perception: New Directions. Annual Review of Psychology. 66, 1 (2015), 173–196. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104.
[71]
Werker, J.F. and Tees, R.C. 1999. Influences on Infant Speech Processing: Toward a New Synthesis. Annual Review of Psychology. 50, 1 (1999), 509–535. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.509.