[1]
Adair, J.C. et al. 1997. Anosognosia: Examining the Disconnection Hypothesis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 63, 6 (1997), 798–800. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.63.6.798.
[2]
Anderson, S.J. et al. 2002. Attentional Processes Link Perception and Action. Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 269, 1497 (2002).
[3]
Bartels, A. et al. 2008. fMRI and Its Interpretations: An Illustration on Directional Selectivity in Area V5/MT. Trends in Neurosciences. 31, 9 (2008), 444–453. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2008.06.004.
[4]
Beck, D.M. et al. 2001. Neural Correlates of Change Detection and Change Blindness. Nature Neuroscience. 4, 6 (2001), 645–650. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/88477.
[5]
Beck, D.M. 2005. Right Parietal Cortex Plays a Critical Role in Change Blindness. Cerebral Cortex. 16, 5 (2005), 712–717. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj017.
[6]
Berti, A. et al. 2008. Motor Awareness and Motor Intention in Anosognosia for Hemiplegia. Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition. Oxford University Press. 163–181.
[7]
Berti, A. et al. 2005. Shared Cortical Anatomy for Motor Awareness and Motor Control. Science. 309, 5733 (2005), 488–491.
[8]
Berti, A. and Bottini, G. 2005. Shared Cortical Anatomy for Motor Awareness and Motor Control. Science. 309, 5733 (2005), 488–491.
[9]
Berti, A. and Rizzolatti, G. 1992. Visual Processing Without Awareness: Evidence From Unilateral Neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 4, 4 (1992), 345–351. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1992.4.4.345.
[10]
Biederman, I. 1972. Perceiving Real-World Scenes. Science. 177, 4043 (1972), 77–80.
[11]
Biran, I. and Chatterjee, A. 2004. Alien Hand Syndrome. Archives of Neurology. 61, 2 (2004).
[12]
Bisiach, E. et al. 1986. Unawareness of Disease Following Lesions of the Right Hemisphere: Anosognosia for Hemiplegia and Anosognosia for Hemianopia. Neuropsychologia. 24, 4 (1986), 471–482. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(86)90092-8.
[13]
Bradshaw, J.L. and Mattingley, J.B. 1995. Clinical Neuropsychology: Behavioral and Brain Science. Clinical Neuropsychology: Behavioral and Brain Science. Academic Press.
[14]
Bradshaw, J.L. and Mattingley, J.B. 1995. Clinical Neuropsychology: Behavioral and Brain Science. Academic Press.
[15]
Bradshaw, J.L. and Mattingley, J.B. 1995. Clinical Neuropsychology: Behavioral and Brain Science. Academic Press.
[16]
Broks, P. 2003. To be Two or Not to Be. Into the Silent Land. Atlantic Books. 204–225.
[17]
Bruce, V. and Young, A. 1986. Understanding Face Recognition. British Journal of Psychology. 77, (1986).
[18]
Buhusi, C.V. and Meck, W.H. 2005. What Makes Us Tick? Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 6, 10 (2005), 755–765. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1764.
[19]
Busigny, T. et al. 2010. Acquired Prosopagnosia as a Face-Specific Disorder: Ruling Out the General Visual Similarity Account. Neuropsychologia. 48, 7 (2010), 2051–2067. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.026.
[20]
Calder, A.J. et al. 2007. Separate Coding of Different Gaze Directions in the Superior Temporal Sulcus and Inferior Parietal Lobule. Current Biology. 17, 1 (2007), 20–25. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.052.
[21]
Calder, A.J. 2011. The Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. Oxford University Press.
[22]
Calder, A.J. and Young, A.W. 2005. Understanding the Recognition of Facial Identity and Facial Expression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 6, 8 (2005), 641–651. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1724.
[23]
Cappa, S. et al. 1987. Remission of Hemineglect and Anosognosia During Vestibular Stimulation. Neuropsychologia. 25, 5 (1987), 775–782. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90115-1.
[24]
Cappa, S. et al. 1987. Remission of Hemineglect and Anosognosia During Vestibular Stimulation. Neuropsychologia. 25, 5 (1987), 775–782. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90115-1.
[25]
Carpenter, K. et al. 1995. Awareness of and Memory for Arm Weakness During Intracarotid Sodium Amytal Testing. Brain. 118, 1 (1995), 243–251. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/118.1.243.
[26]
Cherry, E.C. 1953. Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, With One and With Two Ears. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 25, 5 (1953), 975–979. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1907229.
[27]
Churchland, P.S. and Ramachandran, V.S. 1996. Filling In: Why Dennett Is Wrong. Perception. Oxford University Press.
[28]
Churchland, P.S. and Ramachandran, V.S. 1996. Filling In: Why Dennett Is Wrong. Perception. Oxford University Press.
[29]
Churchland, P.S. and Ramachandran, V.S. 1993. Filling In: Why Dennett Is Wrong. Perception. Oxford University Press. 132–157.
[30]
Cisek, P. and Kalaska, J.F. 2004. Neural Correlates of Mental Rehearsal in Dorsal Premotor Cortex. Nature. 431, 7011 (2004), 993–996. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03005.
[31]
Cocchini, G. et al. 2002. Chronic Anosognosia: A Case Report and Theoretical Account. Neuropsychologia. 40, 12 (2002), 2030–2038. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00054-4.
[32]
Cohen, M.S. et al. 1996. Changes in Cortical Activity During Mental Rotation: A Mapping Study Using Functional MRI. Brain. 119, 1 (1996), 89–100.
[33]
Consciousness: More like Fame than Television: 1996. http://pp.kpnet.fi/seirioa/cdenn/concfame.htm.
[34]
Crick, F. and Koch, C. 1998. Consciousness and Neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991). 8, 2 (1998).
[35]
Cutting, J. 1978. Study of Anosognosia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 41, 6 (1978), 548–555. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.41.6.548.
[36]
Davies, M. et al. 2005. Anosognosia and the Two-Factor Theory of Delusions. Mind and Language. 20, 2 (2005), 209–236. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0268-1064.2005.00283.x.
[37]
Della Sala, S. et al. 1991. Right-Sided Anarchic (Alien) Hand: A Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychologia. 29, 11 (1991), 1113–1127. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(91)90081-I.
[38]
Dennet, D. 1982. Where am I. The Mind’s I. Penguin. 217–231.
[39]
Dennett, D. 1992. Filling in Versus Finding Out: A Ubiquitous Confusion in Cognitive Science. Cognition: conceptual and methodological issues. American Psychological Association.
[40]
Dennett, D. 2007. The Illusion of Consciousness.
[41]
Dennett Daniel, C. 1992. Filling in Versus Finding Out: A Ubiquitous Confusion in Cognitive Science. Cognition, Conception, and Methodological Issues. American Psychological Association.
[42]
Dennett, D.C. 1991. Consciousness Explained. Allen Lane.
[43]
Dresler, M. et al. 2012. Neural Correlates of Dream Lucidity Obtained from Contrasting Lucid versus Non-Lucid REM Sleep: A Combined EEG/fMRI Case Study. Sleep. 35, 7 (Jul. 2012), 1017–1020. DOI:https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.1974.
[44]
Driver, J. et al. 1999. Gaze Perception Triggers Reflexive Visuospatial Orienting. Visual Cognition. 6, 5 (1999), 509–540. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/135062899394920.
[45]
Duncan, J. et al. 1997. Competitive Brain Activity in Visual Attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 7, 2 (1997), 255–261. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80014-1.
[46]
Eimer, M. and Schlaghecken, F. 1998. Effects of Masked Stimuli on Motor Activation: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 24, 6 (1998), 1737–1747. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.6.1737.
[47]
Eimer, M. and Schlaghecken, F. 2002. Links Between Conscious Awareness and Response Inhibition: Evidence From Masked Priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 9, 3 (2002), 514–520. DOI:https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196307.
[48]
Eimer, M. and Schlaghecken, F. 2003. Response Facilitation and Inhibition in Subliminal Priming. Biological Psychology. 64, 1–2 (2003), 7–26. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00100-5.
[49]
Eramudugolla, R. et al. 2005. Directed Attention Eliminates ‘Change Deafness’ in Complex Auditory Scenes. Current Biology. 15, 12 (2005), 1108–1113. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.051.
[50]
Eriksen, C.W. and St. James, J.D. 1986. Visual Attention Within and Around the Field of Focal Attention: A Zoom Lens Model. Perception & Psychophysics. 40, 4 (1986), 225–240. DOI:https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211502.
[51]
Ester, E.F. et al. 2009. Spatially Global Representations in Human Primary Visual Cortex during Working Memory Maintenance. Journal of Neuroscience. 29, 48 (2009), 15258–15265. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4388-09.2009.
[52]
Fasold, O. et al. 2002. Human Vestibular Cortex as Identified With Caloric Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. NeuroImage. 17, 3 (2002), 1384–1393. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1241.
[53]
Fasold, O. et al. 2002. Human Vestibular Cortex as Identified With Caloric Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. NeuroImage. 17, 3 (2002), 1384–1393. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1241.
[54]
Fei-Fei, A. et al. 2007. What Do We Perceive in a Glance of a Real-World Scene?[open access]. Journal of Vision. 7, 1 (2007).
[55]
Findlay, J.M. and Gilchrist, I.D. 2003. Active Vision: The Psychology of Looking and Seeing. Oxford University Press.
[56]
Findlay, J.M. and Gilchrist, I.D. 2003. Active Vision: The Psychology of Looking and Seeing. Oxford University Press.
[57]
Finke, R.A. and Kurtzman, H.S. 1981. Mapping the Visual Field in Mental Imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 110, 4 (1981), 501–517. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.110.4.501.
[58]
Fotopoulou, A. et al. 2010. Implicit Awareness in Anosognosia for Hemiplegia: Unconscious Interference Without Conscious Re-Representation. Brain. 133, 12 (2010), 3564–3577. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq233.
[59]
Fotopoulou, A. et al. 2009. Self-Observation Reinstates Motor Awareness in Anosognosia for Hemiplegia. Neuropsychologia. 47, 5 (2009), 1256–1260. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.018.
[60]
Freisen, C.K. and Kingstone, A. 1999. Inhibition of Return and Reflexive Attention to Gaze Direction [open access]. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. (1999).
[61]
Freudian dream theory today | The Psychologist: https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-13/edition-12/freudian-dream-theory-today-0.
[62]
Friesen, C.K. and Kingstone, A. 1998. The Eyes Have It! Reflexive Orienting Is Triggered by Nonpredictive Gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 5, 3 (1998), 490–495. DOI:https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208827.
[63]
Frith, C.D. et al. 2000. Abnormalities in the Awareness and Control of Action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 355, 1404 (2000), 1771–1788. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2000.0734.
[64]
Furl, N. 2002. Face Recognition Algorithms and the Other-Race Effect: Computational Mechanisms for a Developmental Contact Hypothesis. Cognitive Science. 26, 6 (2002), 797–815. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0364-0213(02)00084-8.
[65]
Garbarini, F. et al. 2012. ‘Moving’ a Paralysed Hand: Bimanual Coupling Effect in Patients With Anosognosia for Hemiplegia. Brain. 135, 5 (2012), 1486–1497. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws015.
[66]
Gold, M. et al. 1994. Anosognosia for Hemiplegia: An Electrophysiologic Investigation of the Feed-Forward Hypothesis. Neurology. 44, 10 (1994), 1804–1804. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.44.10.1804.
[67]
Goldstein, E.B. 2015. Cengage Learning. Sensation and Perception. Wadsworth.
[68]
Goldstein, E.B. 2015. Cengage Learning. Sensation and Perception.
[69]
Goldstein, R.N. 2006. Extract - Identity Crisis. Betraying Spinoza: The Renegade Jew Who Gave Us Modernity. Nextbook/Shocken. 124–125.
[70]
Grèzes, J. and Tucker, M. 2003. Objects Automatically Potentiate Action: An fMRI Study of Implicit Processing. European Journal of Neuroscience. 17, 12 (2003), 2735–2740. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02695.x.
[71]
Grondin, S. 2010. Timing and Time Perception: A Review of Recent Behavioral and Neuroscience Findings and Theoretical Directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 72, 3 (2010), 561–582. DOI:https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.3.561.
[72]
de Haan, B. et al. 2012. Mechanisms and Anatomy of Unilateral Extinction After Brain Injury. Neuropsychologia. 50, 6 (2012), 1045–1053. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.015.
[73]
Halligan, P.W. 1997. Completion in Visuo-Spatial Neglect: A Case Study. Neurocase. 3, 5 (1997), 395–403. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/neucas/3.5.395-j.
[74]
Haxby, J.V. et al. 2000. The Distributed Human Neural System for Face Perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 4, 6 (2000), 223–233. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0.
[75]
Haynes, J.D. 2015. An Information-Based Approach to Consciousness: Mental State Decoding. (2015).
[76]
Haynes, J.D. 2015. An Information-Based Approach to Consciousness: Mental State Decoding. (2015).
[77]
Haynes, J.-D. 2009. Decoding Visual Consciousness From Human Brain Signals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 13, 5 (2009), 194–202. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.02.004.
[78]
Haynes, J.-D. 2009. Decoding Visual Consciousness From Human Brain Signals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 13, 5 (2009), 194–202. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.02.004.
[79]
Heilman, K.M. et al. 1998. Possible Mechanisms of Anosognosia: A Defect in Self–awareness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 353, 1377 (1998), 1903–1909. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1998.0342.
[80]
Heilman, K.M. 2014. Possible Mechanisms of Anosognosia of Hemiplegia. Cortex. 61, (2014), 30–42. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.06.007.
[81]
Henderson, J. 2003. Human Gaze Control During Real-World Scene Perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7, 11 (2003), 498–504. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.006.
[82]
Henderson, J.M. et al. 1999. The Effects of Semantic Consistency on Eye Movements During Complex Scene Viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 25, 1 (1999), 210–228.
[83]
Henderson, J.M. 1992. Visual Attention and Eye Movement Control During Reading  and Picture Viewing. Eye Movements and Visual Cognition: Scene Perception and Reading. Springer.
[84]
Henderson, J.M. and Hollingworth, A. 1999. High-Level Scene Perception. Annual Review of Psychology. 50, 1 (1999), 243–271. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.243.
[85]
Henderson, J.M. and Hollingworth, A. 1999. High-Level Scene Perception. Annual Review of Psychology. 50, 1 (1999), 243–271. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.243.
[86]
Hermens, F. et al. 2010. Inhibition of Masked Primes as Revealed by Saccade Curvature. Vision Research. 50, 1 (2010), 46–56. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.10.008.
[87]
von Hippel, W. and Trivers, R. 2011. The Evolution and Psychology of Self-Deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 34, 1 (2011), 1–16. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10001354.
[88]
Hobson, J.A. et al. 2000. Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience  of conscious states. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23, 6 (Dec. 2000), 793–842. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00003976.
[89]
Hobson, J.A. 2009. REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 10, 11 (Nov. 2009), 803–813. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2716.
[90]
Hofstadter, D. 1945. Prelude… Ant Fugue. Gödel, Escher, Bach : An Eternal Golden Braid. Basic Books. 275–336.
[91]
Hole, G. and Bourne, V. 2010. Face Processing: Psychological, Neuropsychological, and Applied Perspectives. Oxford University Press.
[92]
Hole, G. and Bourne, V. 2010. Face Processing: Psychological, Neuropsychological, and Applied Perspectives. Oxford University Press.
[93]
Howard, R.J. 1998. The Functional Anatomy of Imagining and Perceiving Colour [open access]. NeuroReport. 9, 6 (1998), 1019–1023.
[94]
Hubbard, T.L. 2010. Auditory Imagery: Empirical Findings. Psychological Bulletin. 136, 2 (2010), 302–329. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018436.
[95]
Humphreys, G.W. and Riddoch, M.J. 2000. One More Cup of Coffee for the Road: Object- Action Assemblies, Response Blocking and Response Capture After Frontal Lobe Damage. Executive Control and the Frontal Lobe: Current Issues. 81–93.
[96]
Husain, M. and Nachev, P. 2007. Space and the Parietal Cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11, 1 (2007), 30–36. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.011.
[97]
Ishai, A. and Sagi, D. 1995. Common Mechanisms of Visual Imagery and Perception. Science. 268, 5218 (1995), 1772–1774.
[98]
Itti, L. and Koch, C. 2000. A Saliency-Based Search Mechanism for Overt and Covert Shifts of Visual Attention. Vision Research. 40, 10–12 (2000), 1489–1506. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00163-7.
[99]
Itti, L. and Koch, C. 2001. Computational Modelling of Visual Attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2, 3 (2001), 194–203. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/35058500.
[100]
Jeannerod, M. and Decety, J. 1995. Mental Motor Imagery: A Window Into the Representational Stages of Action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 5, 6 (1995), 727–732. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4388(95)80099-9.
[101]
Jenkins, R. and Beaver, J.D. 2006. I Thought You Were Looking at Me: Direction-Specific Aftereffects in Gaze Perception. Psychological Science. 17, 6 (2006), 506–513.
[102]
Johansson, P. et al. 2005. Failure to Detect Mismatches Between Intention and Outcome in a Simple Decision Task. Science. 310, 5745 (2005), 116–119.
[103]
Johnston, A. et al. 2006. Spatially Localized Distortions of Event Time. Current Biology. 16, 5 (2006), 472–479. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.01.032.
[104]
Kanwisher, N. et al. 1997. The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception [open access]. Journal Of Neuroscience : The Official Journal Of The Society For Neuroscience. 17, 11 (1997).
[105]
Kanwisher, N. and Yovel, G. 2006. The Fusiform Face Area: A Cortical Region Specialized for the Perception of Faces. Philosophical Transactions Of The Royal Society Of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 361, 1476 (2006).
[106]
Karnath, H.-O. 2005. Awareness of the Functioning of One’s Own Limbs Mediated by the Insular Cortex? Journal of Neuroscience. 25, 31 (2005), 7134–7138. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1590-05.2005.
[107]
Karremans, J.C. et al. 2006. Beyond Vicary’s Fantasies: The Impact of Subliminal Priming and Brand Choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 42, 6 (2006), 792–798. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.002.
[108]
KATJA VALLI and ANTTI REVONSUO 2009. The threat simulation theory in light of recent empirical evidence: A review. The American Journal of Psychology. 122, 1 (2009).
[109]
Kingstone, A. 2009. Taking a Real Look at Social Attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 19, 1 (2009), 52–56. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2009.05.004.
[110]
Kosslyn, S.M. 1994. Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate. MIT.
[111]
Kosslyn, S.M. et al. 2001. Neural Foundations of Imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2, 9 (2001), 635–642. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/35090055.
[112]
Kosslyn, S.M. et al. 2001. Neural Foundations of Imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2, 9 (2001), 635–642. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/35090055.
[113]
Kosslyn, S.M. et al. 1995. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Mental Imagery. Neuropsychologia. 33, 11 (1995), 1335–1344. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00067-D.
[114]
Kosslyn, S.M. 1999. The Role of Area 17 in Visual Imagery: Convergent Evidence from PET and rTMS. Science. 284, 5411 (1999), 167–170. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.284.5411.167.
[115]
Kosslyn, S.M. et al. 1995. Topographical Representations of Mental Images in Primary Visual Cortex. Nature. 378, 6556 (1995), 496–498. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/378496a0.
[116]
Kosslyn, S.M. and Thompson, W.L. 2003. When Is Early Visual Cortex Activated During Visual Mental Imagery? Psychological Bulletin. 129, 5 (2003), 723–746. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.723.
[117]
Kuhl, P.K. and Williams, K.A. 1992. Linguistic Experience Alters Phonetic Perception in Infants by 6 Months of Age. Science. 255, 5044 (1992), 606–608.
[118]
Kurzban, R. 2012. Why Everyone (Else) Is a Hypocrite: Evolution and the Modular Mind. Princeton University Press.
[119]
Kurzban, R. 2010. Why Everyone (Else) Is a Hypocrite: Evolution and the Modular Mind.
[120]
Lamba, S. and Nityananda, V. 2014. Self-Deceived Individuals Are Better at Deceiving Others. PLoS ONE. 9, 8 (2014). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104562.
[121]
Langton, S.R.H. et al. 2000. Do the Eyes Have It? Cues to the Direction of Social Attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 4, 2 (2000), 50–59. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01436-9.
[122]
Lee, K. et al. 2000. Caricature Effects, Distinctiveness, and Identification: Testing the Face-Space Framework. Psychological Science. 11, 5 (2000), 379–385.
[123]
van der Leer, L. and McKay, R. 2017. The Optimist Within? Selective Sampling and Self-Deception. Consciousness and Cognition. 50, (2017), 23–29. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.07.005.
[124]
Leopold, D.A. and Logothetis, N.K. 1999. Multistable Phenomena: Changing Views in Perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 3, 7 (1999), 254–264. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01332-7.
[125]
Levine, D.N. 1990. Unawareness of Visual and Sensorimotor Defects: A Hypothesis. Brain and Cognition. 13, 2 (1990), 233–281. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(90)90052-P.
[126]
Light, L.L. et al. 1979. Recognition Memory for Typical and Unusual Faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory. 5, 3 (1979), 212–228. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.5.3.212.
[127]
Linden, D.E.J. et al. 2011. The Brain’s Voices: Comparing Nonclinical Auditory Hallucinations and Imagery. Cerebral Cortex. 21, 2 (2011), 330–337. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq097.
[128]
Loftus, G.R. and Mackworth, N.H. 1978. Cognitive Determinants of Fixation Location During Picture Viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 4, 4 (1978), 565–572. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.4.4.565.
[129]
Lu, H.L. et al. 2000. Proprioception More Impaired Distally Than Proximally in Subjects With Hemispheric Dysfunction. Neurology. 55, 4 (2000), 596–597.
[130]
Lu, L.H. et al. 1997. Anosognosia and Confabulation During the Wada Test. Neurology. 49, 5 (1997), 1316–1322. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.49.5.1316.
[131]
Marcel, A. et al. 2004. Anosognosia for Plegia: Specificity, Extension, Partiality and Disunity of Bodily Unawareness. Cortex. 40, 1 (2004), 19–40. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70919-5.
[132]
Marcel, A.J. 1993. Slippage in the Unity of Consciousness. Experimental and Theoretical Studies of Consciousness. Wiley.
[133]
Marcel, A.J. 1993. Slippage in the Unity of Consciousness. Experimental and Theoretical Studies of Consciousness. Wiley. 168–186.
[134]
Mark Solms 2004. Freud Returns. Scientific American. 290, 5 (2004).
[135]
Mark, V.W. 2007. Alien Hand Syndrome. MedLink Journal.
[136]
Marshall, J.C. and Halligan, P.W. 1988. Blindsight and Insight in Visuo-Spatial Neglect. Nature. 336, 6201 (1988), 766–767. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/336766a0.
[137]
Mattingley, J.B. et al. 1997. Attentional Competition Between Modalities: Extinction Between Touch and Vision After Right Hemisphere Damage. Neuropsychologia. 35, 6 (1997), 867–880. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00008-0.
[138]
McConkie, G.W. and Currie, C.B. 1996. Visual Stability Across Saccades While Viewing Complex Pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 22, 3 (1996), 563–581. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.22.3.563.
[139]
McKay, R. 2011. Isn’t it ironic? A review of ‘Why Everyone (Else) is a Hypocrite: Evolution and the Modular Mind’. Evolution and Human Behavior. 32, 6 (2011), 444–446. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.04.005.
[140]
McKay, R. et al. 2013. Vestibular Stimulation Attenuates Unrealistic Optimism. Cortex. 49, 8 (2013), 2272–2275. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.04.005.
[141]
McKay, R. et al. 2013. Vestibular Stimulation Attenuates Unrealistic Optimism. Cortex. 49, 8 (2013), 2272–2275. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.04.005.
[142]
McNeil, J.E. and Warrington, E.K. 1993. Prosopagnosia: A Face-Specific Disorder. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 46, 1 (1993), 1–10. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749308401064.
[143]
Mele, A.R. 1997. Real self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 20, 1 (1997), 91–102. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X97000034.
[144]
Milner, A.D. and Goodale, M.A. 2006. The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford University Press.
[145]
Milner, A.D. and Goodale, M.A. 2006. The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford University Press, Incorporated.
[146]
Misirlisoy, E. and Hermens, F. 2014. Spatial Primes Produce Dissociated Inhibitory Effects on Saccadic Latencies and Trajectories. Vision Research. 96, (2014), 1–7. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2013.12.017.
[147]
Moscovitch, M. et al. 1997. What Is Special about Face Recognition? Nineteen Experiments on a Person with Visual Object Agnosia and Dyslexia but Normal Face Recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 9, 5 (1997), 555–604. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.555.
[148]
Nardone, I.B. et al. 2008. Attention and Emotion in Anosognosia: Evidence of Implicit Awareness and Repression? Neurocase. 13, 5–6 (2008), 438–445. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/13554790701881749.
[149]
Nathanson, M. 1952. Denial of Illness: Its Occurrence in One Hundred Cases of Hemiplegia. A.M.A. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry. 68, 3 (1952), 380–387. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1001/archneurpsyc.1952.02320210090010.
[150]
Nir, Y. and Tononi, G. 2010. Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 14, 2 (Feb. 2010), 88–100. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.001.
[151]
Nummenmaa, L. and Calder, A.J. 2009. Neural Mechanisms of Social Attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 13, 3 (2009), 135–143. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.12.006.
[152]
O’Regan, J.K. ‘Mud Splashes’ Render Picture Changes Invisible [open access]. Invest. Opthamol. Vis. Sci. 37, 3.
[153]
Orfei, M.D. et al. 2007. Anosognosia for Hemiplegia After Stroke Is a Multifaceted Phenomenon: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Brain. 130, 12 (2007), 3075–3090. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm106.
[154]
Parkhurst, D. et al. 2002. Modeling the Role of Salience in the Allocation of Overt Visual Attention. Vision Research. 42, 1 (2002), 107–123. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00250-4.
[155]
Parsons, L.M. 1987. Imagined Spatial Transformations of One’s Hands and Feet. Cognitive Psychology. 19, 2 (1987), 178–241. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(87)90011-9.
[156]
Paus, T. and Kalina, M. 1991. Medial vs Lateral Frontal Lobe Lesions and Differential Impairment of Central-Gaze Fixation Maintenance in Man. Brain. 114, 5 (1991), 2051–2067. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.5.2051.
[157]
Pearson, J. and Kosslyn, S.M. 2015. The Heterogeneity of Mental Representation: Ending the Imagery Debate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 112, 33 (2015), 10089–10092. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1504933112.
[158]
Pearson, J. and Kosslyn, S.M. 2015. The Heterogeneity of Mental Representation: Ending the Imagery Debate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 112, 33 (2015), 10089–10092. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1504933112.
[159]
Perky, C.W. 1910. An Experimental Study of Imagination. The American Journal of Psychology. 21, 3 (1910), 422–452. DOI:https://doi.org/10.2307/1413350.
[160]
Perrett, D.I. et al. 1982. Visual Neurones Responsive to Faces in the Monkey Temporal Cortex. Experimental Brain Research. 47, 3 (1982). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239352.
[161]
Pia, L. et al. 2004. The Anatomy of Anosognosia for Hemiplegia: A Meta-Analysis. Cortex. 40, 2 (2004), 367–377. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70131-X.
[162]
Pia, L. et al. 2004. The Anatomy of Anosognosia for Hemiplegia: A Meta-Analysis. Cortex. 40, 2 (2004), 367–377. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70131-X.
[163]
Pylyshyn, Z. 2003. Return of the Mental Image: Are There Really Pictures in the Brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7, 3 (2003), 113–118. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00003-2.
[164]
Pylyshyn, Z. 2003. Return of the Mental Image: Are There Really Pictures in the Brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7, 3 (2003), 113–118. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00003-2.
[165]
Ramachandran, V.S. 1995. Anosognosia in Parietal Lobe Syndrome. Consciousness and Cognition. 4, 1 (1995), 22–51. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1995.1002.
[166]
Ramachandran, V.S. 1995. Anosognosia in Parietal Lobe Syndrome. Consciousness and Cognition. 4, 1 (1995), 22–51. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1995.1002.
[167]
Rensink, R.A. 2002. Change Detection. Annual Review of Psychology. 53, 1 (2002), 245–277. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135125.
[168]
Rensink, R.A. 2002. Change Detection. Annual Review of Psychology. 53, 1 (2002), 245–277. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135125.
[169]
Rensink, R.A. et al. 1997. To See or Not to See: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes. Psychological Science. 8, 5 (1997), 368–373.
[170]
Revonsuo, A. 2000. The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23, 6 (Dec. 2000), 877–901. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00004015.
[171]
Revonsuo, Antti The Avatars in the Machine: Dreaming as a Simulation of Social Reality. DOI:https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570375.
[172]
Rich, A.N. et al. 2006. Neural Correlates of Imagined and Synaesthetic Colours. Neuropsychologia. 44, 14 (2006), 2918–2925. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.024.
[173]
Riddoch, M.J. and Edwards, M.G. 1998. Visual Affordances Direct Action: Neuropsychological Evidence From Manual Interference. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 15, 6–8 (1998), 645–683. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/026432998381041.
[174]
Righart, R. and De Gelder, B. 2008. Recognition of Facial Expressions Is Influenced by Emotional Scene Gist. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 8, 3 (2008), 264–272. DOI:https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.8.3.264.
[175]
Rizzolatti, G. and Berti, A. 2013. Neural Mechanisms of Spatial Neglect. Unilateral Neglect. L.Erlbaum. 87–106.
[176]
Robertson, I.H. and Heutink, J. 2002. Rehabilitation of Unilateral Neglect. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: A Cognitive Approach. Boom.
[177]
Robertson, I.H. and Marshall, J.C. 1993. Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies. Erlbaum.
[178]
Rode, G. et al. 1992. Partial Remission of Hemiplegia and Somatoparaphrenia Through Vestibular Stimulation in a Case of Unilateral Neglect. Cortex. 28, 2 (1992), 203–208. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80048-2.
[179]
Rode, G. et al. 1992. Partial Remission of Hemiplegia and Somatoparaphrenia Through Vestibular Stimulation in a Case of Unilateral Neglect. Cortex. 28, 2 (1992), 203–208. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80048-2.
[180]
Rousselet, G. et al. 2005. How Long to Get to the "Gist” of Real-World Natural Scenes? Visual Cognition. 12, 6 (2005), 852–877. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280444000553.
[181]
Sato, W. et al. 2007. Attentional Shift by Gaze Is Triggered Without Awareness. Experimental Brain Research. 183, 1 (2007), 87–94. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1025-x.
[182]
Schlaghecken, F. et al. 2009. Either or Neither, but Not Both: Locating the Effects of Masked Primes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 276, 1656 (2009), 515–521. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0933.
[183]
Sergent, J. and Signoret, J.L. 1992. Varieties of Functional Deficits in Prosopagnosia. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991). 2, 5 (1992).
[184]
Sharot, T. et al. 2011. How Unrealistic Optimism Is Maintained in the Face of Reality. Nature Neuroscience. 14, 11 (2011), 1475–1479. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2949.
[185]
Shepard, R.N. and Metzler, J. 1971. Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects. Science. 171, 3972 (1971), 701–703.
[186]
Siclari, F. et al. 2017. The neural correlates of dreaming. Nature Neuroscience. 20, 6 (Jun. 2017), 872–878. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4545.
[187]
Silverman, M.S. et al. 1982. Deoxyglucose Analysis of Retinotopic Organization in Primate Striate Cortex. Science. 218, 4575 (1982), 902–904.
[188]
Simons, D.J. and Ambinder, M.S. 2005. Change Blindness: Theory and Consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 14, 1 (2005), 44–48.
[189]
Simons, D.J. and Rensink, R.A. 2005. Change Blindness: Past, Present, and Future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9, 1 (2005), 16–20. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.006.
[190]
Singh-Curry, V. and Husain, M. 2010. Rehabilitation of Neglect. Cognitive Neurorehabilitation: Evidence and Application. Cambridge University Press. 449–463.
[191]
Small, M. and Ellis, S. 1996. Denial of Hemiplegia: An Investigation Into the Theories of Causation. European Neurology. 36, 6 (1996), 353–363. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1159/000117293.
[192]
Solms, M. 2000. Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different  brain mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23, 6 (Dec. 2000), 843–850. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00003988.
[193]
Southwick, S.M. et al. 2002. Relationship of Enhanced Norepinephrine Activity During Memory Consolidation to Enhanced Long-Term Memory in Humans. American Journal of Psychiatry. 159, 8 (Aug. 2002), 1420–1422. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1420.
[194]
Stickgold, R. 2000. Replaying the Game: Hypnagogic Images in Normals and Amnesics. Science. 290, 5490 (Oct. 2000), 350–353. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.290.5490.350.
[195]
Sumner, P. and Husain, M. 2007. At the Edge of Consciousness: Automatic Motor Activation and Voluntary Control. The Neuroscientist. 14, 5 (2007), 474–486. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858408314435.
[196]
Sumner, P. and Husain, M. 2007. At the Edge of Consciousness: Automatic Motor Activation and Voluntary Control. The Neuroscientist. 14, 5 (2007), 474–486. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858408314435.
[197]
Sumner, P. and Nachev, P. 2007. Human Medial Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconscious Inhibition of Voluntary Action. Neuron. 54, 5 (2007), 697–711. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.016.
[198]
Tanaka, J.W. and Simonyi, D. 2016. The "Parts and Wholes” of Face Recognition: A Review of the Literature. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 69, 10 (2016), 1876–1889. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1146780.
[199]
Tappin, B.M. et al. 2017. The Heart Trumps the Head: Desirability Bias in Political Belief Revision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 146, 8 (2017), 1143–1149. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000298.
[200]
Tatler, B.W. et al. 2005. Visual Correlates of Fixation Selection: Effects of Scale and Time. Vision Research. 45, 5 (2005), 643–659. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2004.09.017.
[201]
Tatler, B.W. and Wade, N.J. 2010. Yarbus, Eye Movements, and Vision. i-Perception. 1, 1 (2010), 7–27. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1068/i0382.
[202]
Thompson, P. 2009. Margaret Thatcher: A New Illusion. Perception. 38, 6 (2009), 483–484.
[203]
Thompson, P. 2009. Margaret Thatcher: A New Illusion. Perception. 38, 6 (2009), 483–484.
[204]
Thorpe, S. et al. 1996. Speed of Processing in the Human Visual System. Nature. 381, 6582 (1996), 520–522. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/381520a0.
[205]
Tootell, R.B.H. et al. 1998. The Retinotopy of Visual Spatial Attention. Neuron. 21, 6 (1998), 1409–1422. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80659-5.
[206]
Torjussen, T. 1978. Visual Processing in Cortically Blind Hemifields. Neuropsychologia. 16, 1 (1978), 15–21. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(78)90038-6.
[207]
Tsao, D.Y. and Freiwald, W.A. 2006. A Cortical Region Consisting Entirely of Face-Selective Cells. Science (New York, N.Y.). 311, 5761 (2006).
[208]
Tsao, D.Y. and Livingstone, M.S. 2008. Mechanisms of Face Perception. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 31, 1 (2008), 411–437. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094238.
[209]
Tse, P.U. et al. 2004. Attention and the Subjective Expansion of Time. Perception & Psychophysics. 66, 7 (2004), 1171–1189. DOI:https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196844.
[210]
Tucker, M. and Ellis, R. 1998. On the Relations Between Seen Objects and Components of Potential Actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 24, 3 (1998), 830–846. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.830.
[211]
Turnbull, O.H. et al. 2014. Anosognosia as Motivated Unawareness: The ‘Defence’ Hypothesis Revisited. Cortex. 61, (2014), 18–29. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.008.
[212]
Valentine, T. 1991. A Unified Account of the Effects of Distinctiveness, Inversion, and Race in Face Recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 43, 2 (1991), 161–204. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749108400966.
[213]
Valentine, T. et al. 2016. Face-Space: A Unifying Concept in Face Recognition Research. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 69, 10 (2016), 1996–2019. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.990392.
[214]
Valentine, T. and Bruce, V. 1986. The Effects of Distinctiveness in Recognising and Classifying Faces. Perception. 15, 5 (1986).
[215]
Vallar, G. et al. 1994. Anatomical Correlates of Visual and Tactile Extinction in Humans: A Clinical Ct Scan Study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 57, 4 (1994), 464–470. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.57.4.464.
[216]
Vallar, G. et al. 1993. Exploring Somatosensory Hemineglect by Vestibular Stimulation. Brain. 116, 1 (1993), 71–86. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/116.1.71.
[217]
Vallar, G. 1998. Spatial Hemineglect in Humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2, 3 (1998), 87–97. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01145-0.
[218]
Vallar, G. 1998. Spatial Hemineglect in Humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2, 3 (1998), 87–97. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01145-0.
[219]
Vallar, G. et al. 1990. Temporary Remission of Left Hemianesthesia After Vestibular Stimulation. a Sensory Neglect Phenomenon. Cortex. 26, 1 (1990), 123–131. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80078-0.
[220]
Valli, K. et al. 2008. Dreams are more negative than real life: Implications for the function of dreaming. Cognition & Emotion. 22, 5 (Aug. 2008), 833–861. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701541591.
[221]
Valli, K. et al. 2005. The threat simulation theory of the evolutionary function of dreaming: Evidence from dreams of traumatized children. Consciousness and Cognition. 14, 1 (Mar. 2005), 188–218. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00019-9.
[222]
Van Essen, D.C. and Maunsell, J.H.R. 1983. Hierarchical Organization and Functional Streams in the Visual Cortex. Trends in Neurosciences. 6, (1983), 370–375. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(83)90167-4.
[223]
Van Leeuwen, D.S.N. 2007. The Spandrels of Self-Deception: Prospects for a Biological Theory of a Mental Phenomenon. Philosophical Psychology. 20, 3 (2007), 329–348. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/09515080701197148.
[224]
Vitevitch, M.S. 2003. Change Deafness: The Inability to Detect Changes Between Two Voices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 29, 2 (2003), 333–342. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.333.
[225]
Volpe, B.T. et al. 1979. Information Processing of Visual Stimuli in an ‘Extinguished’ Field. Nature. 282, 5740 (1979), 722–724. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/282722a0.
[226]
Vuilleumier, P. 2004. Anosognosia: The Neurology of Beliefs and Uncertainties. Cortex. 40, 1 (2004), 9–17. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70918-3.
[227]
Walker, R. and Mattingley, J.B. 1997. Ghosts in the Machine? Pathological Visual Completion Phenomena in the Damaged Brain. Neurocase. 3, 5 (1997), 313–335. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/13554799708411972.
[228]
Walker, R. and Mattingley, J.B. 1997. Ghosts in the Machine? Pathological Visual Completion Phenomena in the Damaged Brain. Neurocase. 3, 5 (1997), 313–335. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/13554799708411972.
[229]
Walker, R. and Young, A.W. 1996. Object-Based Neglect: An Investigation of the Contributions of Eye Movements and Perceptual Completion. Cortex. 32, 2 (1996), 279–295.
[230]
Walker, R. and Young, A.W. 1996. Object-Based Neglect: An Investigation of the Contributions of Eye Movements and Perceptual Completion. Cortex. 32, 2 (1996), 279–295. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(96)80051-7.
[231]
Walker-Smith, G.J. et al. 1977. Eye Movement Strategies Involved in Face Perception. Perception. 6, 3 (1977).
[232]
Weil, R.S. et al. 2009. Neural Correlates of Hemianopic Completion Across the Vertical Meridian. Neuropsychologia. 47, 2 (2009), 457–464. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.020.
[233]
Weil, R.S. et al. 2009. Neural Correlates of Hemianopic Completion Across the Vertical Meridian. Neuropsychologia. 47, 2 (2009), 457–464. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.020.
[234]
Weinstein, E.A. 1991. Anosognosia and Denial of Illness. Awareness of Deficit After Brain Injury. Oxford University Press. 240–257.
[235]
Weinstein, E.A. and Kahn, R.L. 1955. Denial of Illness: Symbolic and Physiological Aspects. Thomas.
[236]
What is a Neural Correlate of Consciousness? 2000. http://consc.net/papers/ncc2.html.
[237]
Williams, B. 1970. The Self and the Future. The Philosophical Review. 79, 2 (1970), 161–180. DOI:https://doi.org/10.2307/2183946.
[238]
Young, A.W. et al. 1987. Configurational Information in Face Perception. Perception. 16, 6 (1987).
[239]
Young, A.W. et al. 1992. Neglect and Visual Recognition. Brain. 115, 1 (1992), 51–71. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/115.1.51.
[240]
Young, A.W. et al. 1992. Neglect and Visual Recognition. Brain. 115, 1 (1992), 51–71. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/115.1.51.
[241]
Zanker, J.M. 2010. Perception as Gateway to the World. Sensation, Perception and Action: An Evolutionary Perspective. Palgrave Macmillan.