[1]
J. Hay and K. Drager, ‘Sociophonetics’, Annual Review of Anthropology, vol. 36, pp. 89–103, 2007 [Online]. Available: https://www.jstor.org/stable/25064946
[2]
M. Kutas and K. D. Federmeier, ‘Electrophysiology Reveals Semantic Memory Use in Language Comprehension’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 4, no. 12, pp. 463–470, 2000, doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01560-6.
[3]
J. J. A. Van Berkum, D. van den Brink, C. M. J. Y. Tesink, M. Kos, and P. Hagoort, ‘The Neural Integration of Speaker and Message’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 580–591, 2008, doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20054.
[4]
K. Johnson, E. A. Strand, and M. D’Imperio, ‘Auditory–visual Integration of Talker Gender in Vowel Perception’, Journal of Phonetics, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 359–384, 1999, doi: 10.1006/jpho.1999.0100.
[5]
M. K. Tanenhaus, M. J. Spivey-Knowlton, K. M. Eberhard, and J. C. Sedivy, ‘Integration of Visual and Linguistic Information in Spoken Language Comprehension’, Science, vol. 268, no. 5217, 1995 [Online]. Available: https://www.jstor.org/stable/2888637
[6]
L. Raviv, A. Meyer, and S. Lev-Ari, ‘Larger Communities Create More Systematic Languages’, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 286, no. 1907, 2019, doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1262.
[7]
A. Senghas, S. Kita, and A. Özyürek, ‘Children Creating Core Properties of Language: Evidence from an Emerging Sign Language in Nicaragua’, Science, vol. 305, no. 5691, pp. 1779–1782, 2004 [Online]. Available: https://www.jstor.org/stable/3837772
[8]
J. L. Singleton and E. L. Newport, ‘When Learners Surpass Their Models: The Acquisition of American Sign Language From Inconsistent Input’, Cognitive Psychology, vol. 49, no. 4, pp. 370–407, 2004, doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.05.001.
[9]
M. C. Corballis, ‘The Gestural Origins of Language: Human Language May Have Evolved From Manual Gestures, Which Survive Today as a “Behavioral Fossil” Coupled to Speech’, American Scientist, vol. 87, no. 2, pp. 138–145, 1999 [Online]. Available: https://www.jstor.org/stable/27857812
[10]
S. Goldin-Meadow and C. Mylander, ‘Spontaneous Sign Systems Created by Deaf Children in Two Cultures’, Nature, vol. 391, no. 6664, pp. 279–281, 1998, doi: 10.1038/34646.
[11]
S. Kirby, H. Cornish, and K. Smith, ‘Cumulative Cultural Evolution in the Laboratory: An Experimental Approach to the Origins of Structure in Human Language’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 105, no. 31, pp. 10681–10686, 2008, doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707835105.
[12]
H. S. Terrace, L. A. Petitto, R. J. Sanders, and T. G. Bever, ‘Can an Ape Create a Sentence?’, Science, vol. 206, no. 4421, pp. 891–902, 1979 [Online]. Available: https://www.jstor.org/stable/1749272
[13]
‘PALS0009 Introduction to Speech Science: Audio signals and systems’. [Online]. Available: https://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/courses/pals0009/week3.php
[14]
‘PALS0009 Introduction to Speech Science: Voice’. [Online]. Available: https://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/courses/pals0009/week4.php
[15]
‘PALS0009 Introduction to Speech Science: Vowels’. [Online]. Available: https://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/courses/pals0009/week5.php
[16]
‘PALS0009 Introduction to Speech Science: Consonants’. [Online]. Available: https://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/courses/pals0009/week6.php
[17]
P. Ladefoged, A Course in Phonetics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1975.
[18]
N. F. Dronkers, O. Plaisant, M. T. Iba-Zizen, and E. A. Cabanis, ‘Paul Broca’s Historic Cases: High Resolution Mr Imaging of the Brains of Leborgne and Lelong’, Brain, vol. 130, no. 5, pp. 1432–1441, 2007, doi: 10.1093/brain/awm042.
[19]
D. Carey, S. Krishnan, M. F. Callaghan, M. I. Sereno, and F. Dick, ‘Functional and Quantitative MRI Mapping of Somatomotor Representations of Human Supralaryngeal Vocal Tract’, Cerebral Cortex, 2017, doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw393.
[20]
J. P. Rauschecker and S. K. Scott, ‘Maps and Streams in the Auditory Cortex: Nonhuman Primates Illuminate Human Speech Processing’, Nature Neuroscience, vol. 12, no. 6, pp. 718–724, 2009, doi: 10.1038/nn.2331.
[21]
G. Hickok and D. Poeppel, ‘The Cortical Organization of Speech Processing’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 8, no. 5, pp. 393–402, 2007, doi: 10.1038/nrn2113.
[22]
E. Kearney and F. H. Guenther, ‘Articulating: The Neural Mechanisms of Speech Production’, Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, vol. 34, no. 9, pp. 1214–1229, 2019, doi: 10.1080/23273798.2019.1589541.
[23]
G. Hickok, ‘Computational Neuroanatomy of Speech Production’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 135–145, 2012, doi: 10.1038/nrn3158.
[24]
K. E. Watkins et al., ‘MRI Analysis of an Inherited Speech and Language Disorder: Structural Brain Abnormalities’, Brain, vol. 125, no. 3, pp. 465–478, 2002, doi: 10.1093/brain/awf057.
[25]
J. Chesters, R. Möttönen, and K. E. Watkins, ‘Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Over Left Inferior Frontal Cortex Improves Speech Fluency in Adults Who Stutter’, Brain, vol. 141, no. 4, pp. 1161–1171, 2018, doi: 10.1093/brain/awy011.
[26]
K. E. Watkins, S. M. Smith, S. Davis, and P. Howell, ‘Structural and Functional Abnormalities of the Motor System in Developmental Stuttering’, Brain, vol. 131, no. 1, pp. 50–59, 2007, doi: 10.1093/brain/awm241.
[27]
K. Rayner, B. R. Foorman, C. A. Perfetti, D. Pesetsky, and M. S. Seidenberg, ‘How Psychological Science Informs the Teaching of Reading’, Psychological Science, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 31–74, 2001 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40062357
[28]
F. R. Manis, M. S. Seidenberg, L. M. Doi, C. McBride-Chang, and A. Petersen, ‘On the Bases of Two Subtypes of Development Dyslexia’, Cognition, vol. 58, no. 2, pp. 157–195, 1996, doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00679-6.
[29]
B. D. McCandliss, L. Cohen, and S. Dehaene, ‘The Visual Word Form Area: Expertise for Reading in the Fusiform Gyrus’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 7, no. 7, pp. 293–299, 2003, doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00134-7.
[30]
A. Castles and M. Coltheart, ‘Is There a Causal Link From Phonological Awareness to Success in Learning to Read?’, Cognition, vol. 91, no. 1, pp. 77–111, 2004, doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00164-1.
[31]
M. S. Seidenberg, ‘The Science of Reading and Its Educational Implications’, Language Learning and Development, vol. 9, no. 4, pp. 331–360, 2013, doi: 10.1080/15475441.2013.812017.
[32]
A. Castles, K. Rastle, and K. Nation, ‘Ending the Reading Wars: Reading Acquisition From Novice to Expert’, Psychological Science in the Public Interest, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 5–51, 2018, doi: 10.1177/1529100618772271.
[33]
C. J. Price and J. T. Devlin, ‘The Interactive Account of Ventral Occipitotemporal Contributions to Reading’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 15, no. 6, pp. 246–253, 2011, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.001.
[34]
S. Dehaene and L. Cohen, ‘The Unique Role of the Visual Word Form Area in Reading’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 15, no. 6, pp. 254–262, 2011, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003.
[35]
J. S. H. Taylor, F. J. Duff, A. M. Woollams, P. Monaghan, and J. Ricketts, ‘How Word Meaning Influences Word Reading’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 322–328, 2015, doi: 10.1177/0963721415574980.
[36]
F. Vinckier and S. Dehaene, ‘Hierarchical Coding of Letter Strings in the Ventral Stream: Dissecting the Inner Organization of the Visual Word-Form System’, Neuron, vol. 55, no. 1, pp. 143–156, 2007, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.031.
[37]
A. M. Woollams, ‘Connectionist Neuropsychology: Uncovering Ultimate Causes of Acquired Dyslexia’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 369, no. 1634, 2013, doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0398.
[38]
S. Dehaene et al., ‘How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language’, Science, vol. 330, no. 6009, pp. 1359–1364, 2010, doi: 10.1126/science.1194140.
[39]
K. Rastle, S. F. McCormick, L. Bayliss, and K. Rastle, ‘Orthography Influences the Perception and Production of Speech.’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, vol. 37, no. 6, pp. 1588–1594, 2011 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2011-17265-001&site=ehost-live
[40]
J. C. Ziegler, L. Ferrand, and M. Montant, ‘Visual Phonology: The Effects of Orthographic Consistency on Different Auditory Word Recognition Tasks’, Memory & Cognition, vol. 32, no. 5, pp. 732–741, 2004, doi: 10.3758/BF03195863.
[41]
A. M. Woollams, ‘SD-Squared: On the Association Between Semantic Dementia and Surface Dyslexia.’, Psychological Review, vol. 114, no. 2, pp. 316–339, 2007 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2007-05396-004&site=ehost-live
[42]
K. Patterson, P. J. Nestor, and T. T. Rogers, ‘Where Do You Know What You Know? the Representation of Semantic Knowledge in the Human Brain’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 8, no. 12, pp. 976–987, 2007, doi: 10.1038/nrn2277.
[43]
J. J. Purcell, J. Shea, and B. Rapp, ‘Beyond the Visual Word Form Area: The Orthography–semantics Interface in Spelling and Reading’, Cognitive Neuropsychology, vol. 31, no. 5–6, pp. 482–510, 2014, doi: 10.1080/02643294.2014.909399.
[44]
A. M. Woollams and K. Patterson, ‘The Consequences of Progressive Phonological Impairment for Reading Aloud’, Neuropsychologia, vol. 50, no. 14, pp. 3469–3477, 2012, doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.020.
[45]
S. Z. Rapcsak et al., ‘Phonological Dyslexia and Dysgraphia: Cognitive Mechanisms and Neural Substrates’, Cortex, vol. 45, no. 5, pp. 575–591, 2009, doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.04.006.
[46]
A. M. Woollams, ‘Connectionist Neuropsychology: Uncovering Ultimate Causes of Acquired Dyslexia’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 369, no. 1634, 2014, doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0398.
[47]
O. Hauk, I. Johnsrude, and F. Pulvermüller, ‘Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex’, Neuron, vol. 41, no. 2, pp. 301–307, 2004, doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9.
[48]
J. R. Binder, R. H. Desai, W. W. Graves, and L. L. Conant, ‘Where Is the Semantic System? A Critical Review and Meta-Analysis of 120 Functional Neuroimaging Studies’, Cerebral Cortex, vol. 19, no. 12, pp. 2767–2796, 2009, doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055.
[49]
J. G. Rueckl et al., ‘Universal Brain Signature of Proficient Reading: Evidence From Four Contrasting Languages’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 112, no. 50, pp. 15510–15515, 2015, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509321112.
[50]
R. Q. Quiroga, L. Reddy, G. Kreiman, C. Koch, and I. Fried, ‘Invariant Visual Representation by Single Neurons in the Human Brain’, Nature, vol. 435, no. 7045, pp. 1102–1107, 2005, doi: 10.1038/nature03687.
[51]
J. S. H. Taylor, ‘The Influence of Consistency, Frequency, and Semantics on Learning to Read: An Artificial Orthography Paradigm.’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, vol. 37, no. 1, pp. 60–76, 2011 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2010-20426-001&site=ehost-live
[52]
M. J. Cortese and J. Schock, ‘Imageability and Age of Acquisition Effects in Disyllabic Word Recognition’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 66, no. 5, pp. 946–972, 2013, doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.722660.
[53]
F. J. Duff and C. Hulme, ‘The Role of Children’s Phonological and Semantic Knowledge in Learning to Read Words’, Scientific Studies of Reading, vol. 16, no. 6, pp. 504–525, 2012, doi: 10.1080/10888438.2011.598199.
[54]
J. Ricketts, K. Nation, and D. V. M. Bishop, ‘Vocabulary Is Important for Some, but Not All Reading Skills’, Scientific Studies of Reading, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 235–257, 2007, doi: 10.1080/10888430701344306.
[55]
S. Trauzettel-Klosinski and K. Dietz, ‘Standardized Assessment of Reading Performance: The New International Reading Speed Texts IReST’, Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science, vol. 53, no. 9, pp. 5452–5461, 2012, doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-8284.
[56]
Q. R. Mano et al., ‘The Role of Left Occipitotemporal Cortex in Reading: Reconciling Stimulus, Task, and Lexicality Effects’, Cerebral Cortex, vol. 23, no. 4, pp. 988–1001, 2013, doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs093.
[57]
K. L. Wheat, P. L. Cornelissen, S. J. Frost, and P. C. Hansen, ‘During Visual Word Recognition, Phonology Is Accessed within 100 ms and May Be Mediated by a Speech Production Code: Evidence from Magnetoencephalography’, Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 15, pp. 5229–5233, 2010 [Online]. Available: https://www.jneurosci.org/content/30/15/5229.short
[58]
J. Tamminen, ‘Lexical Consolidation’, in International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edition., J. D. Wright, Ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 2015, pp. 920–925 [Online]. Available: http://eu.alma.exlibrisgroup.com/view/action/uresolver.do?operation=resolveService&package_service_id=13408681890002671&institutionId=2671&customerId=2670
[59]
M. P. Walker, ‘Sleep to Remember’, American Scientist, vol. 94, no. 4, pp. 326–333, 2006 [Online]. Available: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1073858406292647
[60]
M. H. Davis and M. G. Gaskell, ‘A Complementary Systems Account of Word Learning: Neural and Behavioural Evidence’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 364, no. 1536, pp. 3773–3800, 2009, doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0111.
[61]
J. Tamminen, M. H. Davis, M. Merkx, and K. Rastle, ‘The Role of Memory Consolidation in Generalisation of New Linguistic Information’, Cognition, vol. 125, no. 1, pp. 107–112, 2012, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.014.
[62]
N. Dumay and M. G. Gaskell, ‘Sleep-Associated Changes in the Mental Representation of Spoken Words’, Psychological Science, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 35–39, 2007 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40064574
[63]
J. Tamminen and M. G. Gaskell, ‘Novel Word Integration in the Mental Lexicon: Evidence From Unmasked and Masked Semantic Priming’, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 66, no. 5, pp. 1001–1025, 2013, doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.724694.
[64]
J. L. McClelland, B. L. McNaughton, and R. C. O’Reilly, ‘Why There Are Complementary Learning Systems in the Hippocampus and Neocortex: Insights From the Successes and Failures of Connectionist Models of Learning and Memory’, Psychological Review, vol. 102, no. 3, pp. 419–457, 1995, doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419.
[65]
J. Tamminen, J. D. Payne, R. Stickgold, E. J. Wamsley, and M. G. Gaskell, ‘Sleep Spindle Activity is Associated with the Integration of New Memories and Existing Knowledge’, Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 43, pp. 14356–14360, 2010 [Online]. Available: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/30/43/14356
[66]
T. Schreiner and B. Rasch, ‘Boosting Vocabulary Learning by Verbal Cueing During Sleep’, Cerebral Cortex, vol. 25, no. 11, pp. 4169–4179, 2015, doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu139.
[67]
L. M. Henderson, A. R. Weighall, H. Brown, and M. Gareth Gaskell, ‘Consolidation of Vocabulary Is Associated With Sleep in Children’, Developmental Science, vol. 15, no. 5, pp. 674–687, 2012, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01172.x.
[68]
S. C. Levinson, ‘Language and Cognition: The Cognitive Consequences of Spatial Description in Guugu Yimithirr’, Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 98–131, 1997, doi: 10.1525/jlin.1997.7.1.98.
[69]
E. Spaepen, M. Coppola, E. S. Spelke, S. E. Carey, and S. Goldin-Meadow, ‘Number Without a Language Model’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 108, no. 8, pp. 3163–3168, 2011, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015975108.
[70]
J. W. Schooler and T. Y. Engstler-Schooler, ‘Verbal Overshadowing of Visual Memories: Some Things Are Better Left Unsaid’, Cognitive Psychology, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 36–71, 1990, doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(90)90003-M.
[71]
J. Winawer, N. Witthoft, M. C. Frank, L. Wu, A. R. Wade, and L. Boroditsky, ‘Russian Blues Reveal Effects of Language on Color Discrimination’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, vol. 104, no. 19, pp. 7780–7785, 2007 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/25427570
[72]
J. A. Lucy and S. Gaskins, ‘Grammatical Categories and the Development of Classification Preferences: A Comparative Approach’, in Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, vol. 3, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001, pp. 257–283.
[73]
E. F. Loftus and J. C. Palmer, ‘Reconstruction of Automobile Destruction: An Example of the Interaction Between Language and Memory.’, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, vol. 13, no. 5, pp. 585–589, 1974, doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3.
[74]
B. Schick, P. de Villiers, J. de Villiers, and R. Hoffmeister, ‘Language and Theory of Mind: A Study of Deaf Children’, Child Development, vol. 78, no. 2, pp. 376–396, 2007, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x.
[75]
M. Ross, W. Q. E. Xun, and A. E. Wilson, ‘Language and the Bicultural Self’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 28, no. 8, pp. 1040–1050, 2002, doi: 10.1177/01461672022811003.
[76]
M. J. Spivey and V. Marian, ‘Cross Talk Between Native and Second Languages: Partial Activation of an Irrelevant Lexicon’, Psychological Science, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 281–284, 1999, doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00151.
[77]
E. Bialystok, F. I. M. Craik, and G. Luk, ‘Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind and Brain’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 16, no. 4, pp. 240–250, 2012, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001.
[78]
V. Cook, ‘Introduction: The Changing L1 in the L2 User’s Mind’, in Effects of the Second Language on the First, vol. 3, Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, 2003, pp. 1–18.
[79]
J. A. Linck, J. F. Kroll, and G. Sunderman, ‘Losing Access to the Native Language While Immersed in a Second Language: Evidence for the Role of Inhibition in Second-Language Learning’, Psychological Science, vol. 20, no. 12, pp. 1507–1515, 2009, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02480.x.
[80]
A. I. Schwartz and J. F. Kroll, ‘Bilingual Lexical Activation in Sentence Context’, Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 55, no. 2, pp. 197–212, 2006, doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.004.
[81]
V. Valian, ‘Bilingualism and Cognition’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 3–24, 2015, doi: 10.1017/S1366728914000522.