Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind and Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where Is the Semantic System? A Critical Review and Meta-Analysis of 120 Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2767–2796. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055
Carey, D., Krishnan, S., Callaghan, M. F., Sereno, M. I., & Dick, F. (2017). Functional and Quantitative MRI Mapping of Somatomotor Representations of Human Supralaryngeal Vocal Tract. Cerebral Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw393
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is There a Causal Link From Phonological Awareness to Success in Learning to Read? Cognition, 91(1), 77–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00164-1
Castles, A., Rastle, K., & Nation, K. (2018). Ending the Reading Wars: Reading Acquisition From Novice to Expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(1), 5–51. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100618772271
Chesters, J., Möttönen, R., & Watkins, K. E. (2018). Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Over Left Inferior Frontal Cortex Improves Speech Fluency in Adults Who Stutter. Brain, 141(4), 1161–1171. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy011
Cook, V. (2003). Introduction: The Changing L1 in the L2 User’s Mind. In Effects of the Second Language on the First (Vol. 3, pp. 1–18). Multilingual Matters.
Corballis, M. C. (1999). The Gestural Origins of Language: Human Language May Have Evolved From Manual Gestures, Which Survive Today as a ‘Behavioral Fossil’ Coupled to Speech. American Scientist, 87(2), 138–145. https://www.jstor.org/stable/27857812
Cortese, M. J., & Schock, J. (2013). Imageability and Age of Acquisition Effects in Disyllabic Word Recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 946–972. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.722660
Davis, M. H., & Gaskell, M. G. (2009). A Complementary Systems Account of Word Learning: Neural and Behavioural Evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3773–3800. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0111
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The Unique Role of the Visual Word Form Area in Reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 254–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003
Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Filho, G. N., Jobert, A., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., & Cohen, L. (2010). How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language. Science, 330(6009), 1359–1364. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1194140
Dronkers, N. F., Plaisant, O., Iba-Zizen, M. T., & Cabanis, E. A. (2007). Paul Broca’s Historic Cases: High Resolution Mr Imaging of the Brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain, 130(5), 1432–1441. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm042
Duff, F. J., & Hulme, C. (2012). The Role of Children’s Phonological and Semantic Knowledge in Learning to Read Words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(6), 504–525. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2011.598199
Dumay, N., & Gaskell, M. G. (2007). Sleep-Associated Changes in the Mental Representation of Spoken Words. Psychological Science, 18(1), 35–39. http://www.jstor.org/stable/40064574
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Mylander, C. (1998). Spontaneous Sign Systems Created by Deaf Children in Two Cultures. Nature, 391(6664), 279–281. https://doi.org/10.1038/34646
Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex. Neuron, 41(2), 301–307. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9
Hay, J., & Drager, K. (2007). Sociophonetics. Annual Review of Anthropology, 36, 89–103. https://www.jstor.org/stable/25064946
Henderson, L. M., Weighall, A. R., Brown, H., & Gareth Gaskell, M. (2012). Consolidation of Vocabulary Is Associated With Sleep in Children. Developmental Science, 15(5), 674–687. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01172.x
Hickok, G. (2012). Computational Neuroanatomy of Speech Production. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(2), 135–145. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3158
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The Cortical Organization of Speech Processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393–402. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2113
Johnson, K., Strand, E. A., & D’Imperio, M. (1999). Auditory–visual Integration of Talker Gender in Vowel Perception. Journal of Phonetics, 27(4), 359–384. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.1999.0100
Kearney, E., & Guenther, F. H. (2019). Articulating: The Neural Mechanisms of Speech Production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(9), 1214–1229. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1589541
Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative Cultural Evolution in the Laboratory: An Experimental Approach to the Origins of Structure in Human Language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(31), 10681–10686. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707835105
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology Reveals Semantic Memory Use in Language Comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463–470. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01560-6
Ladefoged, P. (1975). A Course in Phonetics. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Levinson, S. C. (1997). Language and Cognition: The Cognitive Consequences of Spatial Description in Guugu Yimithirr. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 7(1), 98–131. https://doi.org/10.1525/jlin.1997.7.1.98
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Losing Access to the Native Language While Immersed in a Second Language: Evidence for the Role of Inhibition in Second-Language Learning. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1507–1515. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02480.x
Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of Automobile Destruction: An Example of the Interaction Between Language and Memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13(5), 585–589. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3
Lucy, J. A., & Gaskins, S. (2001). Grammatical Categories and the Development of Classification Preferences: A Comparative Approach. In Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development (Vol. 3, pp. 257–283). Cambridge University Press.
Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Petersen, A. (1996). On the Bases of Two Subtypes of Development Dyslexia. Cognition, 58(2), 157–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00679-6
Mano, Q. R., Humphries, C., Desai, R. H., Seidenberg, M. S., Osmon, D. C., Stengel, B. C., & Binder, J. R. (2013). The Role of Left Occipitotemporal Cortex in Reading: Reconciling Stimulus, Task, and Lexicality Effects. Cerebral Cortex, 23(4), 988–1001. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs093
McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The Visual Word Form Area: Expertise for Reading in the Fusiform Gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 293–299. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00134-7
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why There Are Complementary Learning Systems in the Hippocampus and Neocortex: Insights From the Successes and Failures of Connectionist Models of Learning and Memory. Psychological Review, 102(3), 419–457. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419
PALS0009 Introduction to Speech Science: Audio signals and systems. (n.d.). https://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/courses/pals0009/week3.php
PALS0009 Introduction to Speech Science: Consonants. (n.d.). https://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/courses/pals0009/week6.php
PALS0009 Introduction to Speech Science: Voice. (n.d.). https://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/courses/pals0009/week4.php
PALS0009 Introduction to Speech Science: Vowels. (n.d.). https://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/courses/pals0009/week5.php
Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where Do You Know What You Know? the Representation of Semantic Knowledge in the Human Brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976–987. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277
Price, C. J., & Devlin, J. T. (2011). The Interactive Account of Ventral Occipitotemporal Contributions to Reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 246–253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.001
Purcell, J. J., Shea, J., & Rapp, B. (2014). Beyond the Visual Word Form Area: The Orthography–semantics Interface in Spelling and Reading. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31(5–6), 482–510. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2014.909399
Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., & Fried, I. (2005). Invariant Visual Representation by Single Neurons in the Human Brain. Nature, 435(7045), 1102–1107. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03687
Rapcsak, S. Z., Beeson, P. M., Henry, M. L., Leyden, A., Kim, E., Rising, K., Andersen, S., & Cho, H. (2009). Phonological Dyslexia and Dysgraphia: Cognitive Mechanisms and Neural Substrates. Cortex, 45(5), 575–591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.04.006
Rastle, K., McCormick, S. F., Bayliss, L., & Rastle, K. (2011). Orthography Influences the Perception and Production of Speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(6), 1588–1594. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2011-17265-001&site=ehost-live
Rauschecker, J. P., & Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and Streams in the Auditory Cortex: Nonhuman Primates Illuminate Human Speech Processing. Nature Neuroscience, 12(6), 718–724. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2331
Raviv, L., Meyer, A., & Lev-Ari, S. (2019). Larger Communities Create More Systematic Languages. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1907). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.1262
Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How Psychological Science Informs the Teaching of Reading. Psychological Science, 2(2), 31–74. http://www.jstor.org/stable/40062357
Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Vocabulary Is Important for Some, but Not All Reading Skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(3), 235–257. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430701344306
Ross, M., Xun, W. Q. E., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). Language and the Bicultural Self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(8), 1040–1050. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672022811003
Rueckl, J. G., Paz-Alonso, P. M., Molfese, P. J., Kuo, W.-J., Bick, A., Frost, S. J., Hancock, R., Wu, D. H., Mencl, W. E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Lee, J.-R., Oliver, M., Zevin, J. D., Hoeft, F., Carreiras, M., Tzeng, O. J. L., Pugh, K. R., & Frost, R. (2015). Universal Brain Signature of Proficient Reading: Evidence From Four Contrasting Languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(50), 15510–15515. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1509321112
Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and Theory of Mind: A Study of Deaf Children. Child Development, 78(2), 376–396. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x
Schooler, J. W., & Engstler-Schooler, T. Y. (1990). Verbal Overshadowing of Visual Memories: Some Things Are Better Left Unsaid. Cognitive Psychology, 22(1), 36–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(90)90003-M
Schreiner, T., & Rasch, B. (2015). Boosting Vocabulary Learning by Verbal Cueing During Sleep. Cerebral Cortex, 25(11), 4169–4179. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu139
Schwartz, A. I., & Kroll, J. F. (2006). Bilingual Lexical Activation in Sentence Context. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 197–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.004
Seidenberg, M. S. (2013). The Science of Reading and Its Educational Implications. Language Learning and Development, 9(4), 331–360. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2013.812017
Senghas, A., Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2004). Children Creating Core Properties of Language: Evidence from an Emerging Sign Language in Nicaragua. Science, 305(5691), 1779–1782. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3837772
Singleton, J. L., & Newport, E. L. (2004). When Learners Surpass Their Models: The Acquisition of American Sign Language From Inconsistent Input. Cognitive Psychology, 49(4), 370–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.05.001
Spaepen, E., Coppola, M., Spelke, E. S., Carey, S. E., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Number Without a Language Model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(8), 3163–3168. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1015975108
Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V. (1999). Cross Talk Between Native and Second Languages: Partial Activation of an Irrelevant Lexicon. Psychological Science, 10(3), 281–284. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00151
Tamminen, J. (2015). Lexical Consolidation. In J. D. Wright (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (2nd Edition, pp. 920–925). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.51044-7
Tamminen, J., Davis, M. H., Merkx, M., & Rastle, K. (2012). The Role of Memory Consolidation in Generalisation of New Linguistic Information. Cognition, 125(1), 107–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.014
Tamminen, J., & Gaskell, M. G. (2013). Novel Word Integration in the Mental Lexicon: Evidence From Unmasked and Masked Semantic Priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 1001–1025. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.724694
Tamminen, J., Payne, J. D., Stickgold, R., Wamsley, E. J., & Gaskell, M. G. (2010). Sleep Spindle Activity is Associated with the Integration of New Memories and Existing Knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(43), 14356–14360. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/30/43/14356
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of Visual and Linguistic Information in Spoken Language Comprehension. Science, 268(5217). https://www.jstor.org/stable/2888637
Taylor, J. S. H. (2011). The Influence of Consistency, Frequency, and Semantics on Learning to Read: An Artificial Orthography Paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(1), 60–76. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2010-20426-001&site=ehost-live
Taylor, J. S. H., Duff, F. J., Woollams, A. M., Monaghan, P., & Ricketts, J. (2015). How Word Meaning Influences Word Reading. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 322–328. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721415574980
Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an Ape Create a Sentence? Science, 206(4421), 891–902. https://www.jstor.org/stable/1749272
Trauzettel-Klosinski, S., & Dietz, K. (2012). Standardized Assessment of Reading Performance: The New International Reading Speed Texts IReST. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science, 53(9), 5452–5461. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.11-8284
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and Cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000522
Van Berkum, J. J. A., van den Brink, D., Tesink, C. M. J. Y., Kos, M., & Hagoort, P. (2008). The Neural Integration of Speaker and Message. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 580–591. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20054
Vinckier, F., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Hierarchical Coding of Letter Strings in the Ventral Stream: Dissecting the Inner Organization of the Visual Word-Form System. Neuron, 55(1), 143–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.031
Walker, M. P. (2006). Sleep to Remember. American Scientist, 94(4), 326–333. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1073858406292647
Watkins, K. E., Smith, S. M., Davis, S., & Howell, P. (2007). Structural and Functional Abnormalities of the Motor System in Developmental Stuttering. Brain, 131(1), 50–59. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm241
Watkins, K. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R. E., Connelly, A., Friston, K. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Mishkin, M., & Gadian, D. G. (2002). MRI Analysis of an Inherited Speech and Language Disorder: Structural Brain Abnormalities. Brain, 125(3), 465–478. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf057
Wheat, K. L., Cornelissen, P. L., Frost, S. J., & Hansen, P. C. (2010). During Visual Word Recognition, Phonology Is Accessed within 100 ms and May Be Mediated by a Speech Production Code: Evidence from Magnetoencephalography. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(15), 5229–5233. https://www.jneurosci.org/content/30/15/5229.short
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., Wade, A. R., & Boroditsky, L. (2007). Russian Blues Reveal Effects of Language on Color Discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, 104(19), 7780–7785. http://www.jstor.org/stable/25427570
Woollams, A. M. (2007). SD-Squared: On the Association Between Semantic Dementia and Surface Dyslexia. Psychological Review, 114(2), 316–339. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2007-05396-004&site=ehost-live
Woollams, A. M. (2013). Connectionist Neuropsychology: Uncovering Ultimate Causes of Acquired Dyslexia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0398
Woollams, A. M. (2014). Connectionist Neuropsychology: Uncovering Ultimate Causes of Acquired Dyslexia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0398
Woollams, A. M., & Patterson, K. (2012). The Consequences of Progressive Phonological Impairment for Reading Aloud. Neuropsychologia, 50(14), 3469–3477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.020
Ziegler, J. C., Ferrand, L., & Montant, M. (2004). Visual Phonology: The Effects of Orthographic Consistency on Different Auditory Word Recognition Tasks. Memory & Cognition, 32(5), 732–741. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195863